Behavioral Genetics | Definition, Types & Examples

Chris has over 20 years of experience in research analysis, program development, management, and adult education. Extensive global experience collaborating with diverse ethnic and cultural backgrounds achieving mutual goals. He has a Bachelor of Science in Sociology from Southern Utah University and a Master of Science in Adult, Occupational, and Continuing Education from Kansas State University. He is currently completing a second Master of Arts in Clinical Mental Health Counseling with Southern New Hampshire University. He also possesses a Diversity Equity and Inclusion in the Workplace Certification from the University of Southern Florida. He has served in the U.S. Army for over 28 years, retiring in 2023.

  • Instructor Ryan Villard

Table of Contents

Behavioral genetics definition, types of studies in behavioral genetics, statistics in behavioral genetics, lesson summary, how do behavioral genetics explain behavior.

Behavioral genetics demonstrate that both genetics and environmental exposure can influence behavior. It proves what behaviors are heavily influenced by genetic traits and which can develop due to environmental exposure.

What do behavior geneticists do?

Behavioral genetics identify the relationship between genetic traits and environmental variables influencing specific behaviors and determine the strengths and weaknesses of these relationships. They focus research on twins and siblings with similar genetic characteristics and analyze ecological relationships and the similarities are differences in behaviors.

What are behavioral genetics?

Behavioral genetics is the study of the influence one's genetic makeup and lived experiences within their unique environment have on their current behavior. Many use the term nature versus nurture to determine if a particular behavior results from nurture, the environmental exposure, or result from nature, the genetic makeup.

The assumption that human behavior is influenced by genes and exposure to the environment in which one interacts is known as behavioral genetics . With its founding credited to the Englishman Sir Francis Galton in the late 19th Century, the field of behavioral genetics examines both genes and environmental exposure to uncover the root cause of certain behaviors. Throughout human development, lived experiences and exposure to the elements within the environment combined with existing genetic traits influence behavior. Behavioral genetics aims to understand how these existing traits, combined with ones lived experiences within their environment, influence specific behaviors. Behavioral genetics helps researchers uncover the root cause influencing certain behaviors.

Examples of Behavioral Genetics

  • Researchers have discovered that the root cause of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is approximately 30% genetic and 70% influenced by past lived experiences. Researchers have also found that some with genetic traits of GAD may never develop anxiety due to the quality of their lived experiences within their environment. Twins separated at birth with the genetic characteristics of GAD may see one twin develop symptoms of GAD while the other may not be due to different environmental exposures.
  • Empirical evidence suggests higher levels of mental disorders within the African American population. Researchers suggest this is due to either genetic traits or exposure to poverty, violence, systemic racism, and increased social stressors found within their environment. Adopted siblings placed in homes of differing social-economic statuses may develop different behavioral traits due to their environmental exposure.
  • Researchers have discovered that the level of educational attainment is only one-sixth genetically passed down to offspring. The most significant factor influencing one's academic ability is their socio-environmental experiences.

To unlock this lesson you must be a Study.com Member. Create your account

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

An error occurred trying to load this video.

Try refreshing the page, or contact customer support.

You must c C reate an account to continue watching

Register to view this lesson.

As a member, you'll also get unlimited access to over 88,000 lessons in math, English, science, history, and more. Plus, get practice tests, quizzes, and personalized coaching to help you succeed.

Get unlimited access to over 88,000 lessons.

Already registered? Log in here for access

Resources created by teachers for teachers.

I would definitely recommend Study.com to my colleagues. It’s like a teacher waved a magic wand and did the work for me. I feel like it’s a lifeline.

You're on a roll. Keep up the good work!

Just checking in. are you still watching.

  • 0:04 Twin Studies
  • 2:00 Statistics in…
  • 3:05 Family Studies
  • 4:15 Adoption Studies
  • 5:20 Lesson Summary

Behaviors, personalities, and psychopathologies are influenced by exposure to the environment or genetic makeup. Researchers have used family, twins, and adoption as data sets to analyze and quantify the environment's effect on behavior over the person's genetic makeup. Research has looked to answer if genetic traits have a more substantial influence on future behavior regardless of the environmental variables.

Twin Studies

Twin studies focus on the behaviors of twins raised in the same family environments and determine if the genetic or environmental variables have more influence on behavior. Variation exists in these studies. Researchers consider whether the twins are identical, also known as monozygotic sharing all of their genes, or fraternal, also known as dizygotic, only sharing 50 percent of them. Researchers analyze the variables of the environment and the unique genetic makeup of the twins to determine if they lived experiences and exposure to the variables within their environment have more influence on behavior or if the effect is due to genetics. There is an advantage of twins' studies and determining whether genetics or environment impacts behavior. This allows researchers to determine if certain behaviors result from genetic makeup or lived experiences within the environment. It enables medical providers to determine the best route for treating psychopathologies such as depression, anxiety, dyslexia, or neuroticism.

Neuroticism is an excellent example of an area where behavioral genetics have focused their efforts. Neuroticism is a personality trait where one cannot regulate the negative emotions that influence constant guilt, depression, and anxiety. When someone is diagnosed with neuroticism, they struggle with regular daily activities due to the inability to regulate the negative emotions associated with everyday stressors. Studies demonstrate that neuroticism is 40-60 percent heritable. Yet other studies indicate that childhood trauma also influences the development of neuroticism. Research would analyze the environmental lived experiences and the family history to determine where the significant influence lies.

Family Studies

Family studies require participants to have at least two siblings with similar genes, helping researchers determine the root cause of certain behaviors. Psychologists and behavior therapists gather information on family history during intake assessments to support the development of their treatment plan. Sometimes a behavior may be a genetic trait. Yet, the symptoms were never prevalent amongst the parents, and the patient may state on the assessment that there is no family history of the problem. For example, some can have the genetic traits for anxiety, yet their lived experiences never influenced the symptoms to the surface. When someone believes that their issues are due to genetic conditions and are the starting point of a genetic study, they are considered a proband. Typically, the proband has no past family evidence of a genetic disorder, and an individual is the first family member to undergo a genetic study. When geneticists look at behavioral traits, they will analyze the genetic characteristics of both first-degree and second-degree relatives. A first-degree relative shares close to half of the genetic markers. This includes siblings, offspring, and parents. Second-degree relatives share about 25% of genetic traits: grandparents, aunts, uncles, grandchildren, nieces, nephews, and half-siblings.

Adoption Studies

Adoption studies are another method of behavioral genetics. A key concept within the study of behavioral genetics that adoption studies provide is the concept of nurture versus nature . Or, in other words, environment versus genetics. Nurture describes the lived experiences and exposure to the variables within one's environment. For example, if a child grew up in an abusive home, they may develop psychological issues as an adult compared to a child in a healthy environment free of abuse. The first child's problems would be a result of poor nurture. Nature results from one's genetic makeup and is passed down from family members. For example, dyslexia is regarded as a genetic condition. A child can be raised in a healthy academic environment and still find difficulty reading and writing because of genetics or nature.

A well-known example of looking specifically at several cognitive traits, behaviors, and interests is found in a 1990 study published in the journal Science titled, "Sources of Human Psychological Differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart." This study examined identical twins' behavior and cognitive traits separated and raised in two distinct environments and twins raised in the same environment. Researchers discovered that genetic markers have a more substantial influence on behavior than the child's environment. This means that identical twins raised apart will demonstrate similar characteristics as identical twins raised together. They even found that genetic factors could control the effects of these ecological variables under strong environmental influences.

In behavioral genetics, researchers use a statistical term called the concordance rate. Concordance rate is defined as "the percentage of pairs of twins or other blood relatives who exhibit a particular trait or disorder." For example, researchers may look at several identical twins separated at birth with hereditary neuroticism and determine the concordance rate of those diagnosed with generalized anxiety disorder. The percentage of twins diagnosed with generalized anxiety would be explained in the concordance percentage.

When analyzing the relationship between two variables influencing behavior, statisticians use the correlation coefficient . A correlation is "a statistical measurement of the relationship between two variables" and allows researchers to compare and contrast different variables that may influence behavior. A correlation coefficient is identified with a number scale from a negative one to a positive one that depicts the strength of the relationship between these two variables. Results are plotted on a graph to show their relationship visually. This measurement results in either a positive, negative, or zero correlation. The farther away a variable is plotted to zero, the stronger the relationship. For example, say researchers find that the symptoms of depression decrease when individuals conduct five hours of daily mediation. As the variable of meditation increases, the symptoms of depression decrease. This is a negative correlation because the two variables travel in different directions. If the daily meditation increases and the signs of depression increase, this would positively correlate as both variables increase in the same direction. If no change is found, this would be a zero correlation as there is no relationship between the two variables. Researchers use the correlation coefficient to compare and contrast behavioral traits between twins and the relationship between genetic and environmental variables.

Throughout human development, genetic makeup combined with lived experiences within one's environment influence the current behavior of the human race. Behavioral genetics helps researchers understand drivers of human behavior by examining the degree of influence these existing genetic traits and lived experiences within the environment have on behaviors. Research studies focus on the specifics of this behavior by concentrating on families, twins, and adoption . There is reason and value behind this research as it helps uncover the cause of human behavior, determining if genetics or environmental variables are the primary cause for specific behavior. Discovering the root cause of behavior also aids medical providers in focusing behavioral treatment on the root cause of the behavior problem.

The Minnesota Twin study is an excellent example of this research. It looked to determine when both identical or fraternal twins raised in the same households are, their behavior is influenced more by genetics or their environment. In these studies, researchers look closely at the genetic and environmental variables to determine if any relationship influences behavior. They look for a correlation coefficient to measure how a particular variable influences a specific trait in one twin compared to the other. Researchers describe the similarities and portions of identical twins suffering from mental illnesses such as depression using the statistical term concordance rate . In this research, family history is examined to look at past similar traits when determining mental illness hereditary traits. In some cases, when there is no previous history of a mental illness and an individual is the starting point of a genetic study, this person is referred to as the proband .

Video Transcript

Compared to your parents, how frequently do you tend to worry, and to experience negative emotions? What evidence is there for personality being at least partially determined by genetics? Consider this example: in the early 1950s, a German-British psychologist named Hans Jurgen Eysenck was the lead author on a published study about whether neuroticism is inherited. Neuroticism is a term that psychologists use to label a general tendency to experience negative emotions . Eysenck's test was a twin study , which used 25 pairs of fraternal twins , and 25 pairs of identical twins . Identical twins have the same genes, whereas fraternal twins have only about half the same genes. So, here's the value of a twin study like this: if identical twins are raised in the same households, and fraternal twins are raised in the same households, then characteristics that are determined largely by environmental factors should be about equally pronounced in both kinds of twins. By contrast, assuming again that both identical twins and fraternal twins are each raised in the same households, characteristics that are more influenced by genetics should be more similar between identical twins than between fraternal ones. In other words, the goal of identical vs. fraternal twin studies is to understand the significance of genetics for a particular characteristic. Identical twins share twice as much genetic code as fraternal twins, so by comparing the degree of similarity between the two different kinds of twins, researchers can better understand the role that genetics play in determining that characteristic. Eysenck's study found that neuroticism seemed to be overwhelmingly determined by genetics, with about 80% stemming from biological factors, and only 20% from environmental ones. This is normal: twin studies like this usually find more similarity between identical than between fraternal twins.

Twin studies such as Eyesenck's belong within a field known as behavioral genetics , which studies the heritability of traits in animals and humans. Behavioral genetics is not exclusively a sub-field of psychology; it's more of an interdisciplinary field that impinges on disciplines including psychology, biology and statistics. And studies in behavioral genetics do rely heavily on statistics. For example, twin studies often must establish concordance rates , which is the proportion of twin pairs in which both individuals exhibit the trait being considered. Now, with some traits, such as red hair, you either have it or you don't. With others, though, such as height, you can exhibit the trait to varying degrees. With these traits that can differ in degree, another type of statistic is needed. It's called a correlation coefficient . You don't need to worry about how to calculate it at the moment; just know that a correlation coefficient measures the extent to which the trait in one twin compares or contrasts with the same trait in the other twin.

Aside from twin studies, another type of research method that behavioral geneticists use to study humans is a family study . Probably you can guess from the name what this type of study is. A family study begins by isolating a single trait, or even a single illness, in a single person. This first person is known as the proband . Then that person's parents, siblings and children are studied with reference to the same trait. These relatives are known as first-degree relatives . Sometimes, family studies will even consider more distant relatives, such as aunts and grandparents, and so forth. These more distant relatives are known as second-degree relatives . Obviously, family studies aim to identify whether a trait runs in a family. But can you think of a complication with them? Families tend not only to be similar in terms of genetic makeup; they also tend to live in similar, if not the same, environments. So even though family studies can determine whether a given trait runs in a given family, they cannot easily determine whether it runs in the family due to environment or due to genetics. Twin studies, such as Eyesenck's, are better for distinguishing that.

Another variation of a family study is an adoption study . Adoption studies can happen in two different ways. First, biologically related people who have been raised in different environments can be studied. For example, what if identical twins were adopted by different families, and raised in different households? Since they have the same DNA, studies like these are well-positioned for drawing conclusions about effects those different environments have exerted on those twins. One well-known example of a study like this is known as the Minnesota Twin Study . It studied a pair of identical twins who were raised separately, and who did not meet until they were 40. The results were astonishing. One researcher concluded, '(On) multiple measures of personality and temperament, occupational and leisure-time interests and social attitudes, (identical) twins reared apart are about as similar as (identical) twins reared together.' Second, adopted children can be compared to their adopted parents, to their biological parents, or both. The Texas Adoption Project is one well-known adoption study.

To summarize, behavioral genetics is a field that attempts to ascertain whether, or the extent to which, traits, including personality traits, are inherited. To help determine this, researchers study families in family studies, twin studies and adoption studies. These studies are heavily reliant on statistical measurements, including concordance rates , which indicate the proportions of pairs in given studies in which both individuals exhibit the trait in question, and correlation coefficients. A correlation coefficient measures the extent to which the prevalence of a trait in one person matches or diverges from the prevalence of the same trait in another.

Lesson Objectives

After watching this lesson, you should be able to:

  • Explain the goal of behavioral genetics
  • Describe twin studies, family studies and adoption studies
  • Define the terms concordance rate and correlation coefficient and understand how they are used in behavioral genetics

Unlock Your Education

See for yourself why 30 million people use study.com, become a study.com member and start learning now..

Already a member? Log In

Recommended Lessons and Courses for You

Related lessons, related courses, recommended lessons for you.

Adoption & Twin Studies | Findings & Examples

Behavioral Genetics | Definition, Types & Examples Related Study Materials

  • Related Topics

Browse by Courses

  • Psychology 315: Psychology of Motivation
  • VCE Psychology: Exam Prep & Study Guide
  • Worth Publishers Psychology: Online Textbook Help
  • Intro to Mass Communications II
  • Psychology 306: Advanced Abnormal Psychology
  • PLACE Special Education Generalist: Practice & Study Guide
  • Psychology of Diversity
  • Communications 301: Diversity and Intercultural Communication
  • Psychology 316: Advanced Social Psychology
  • Disability Awareness & Etiquette in the Workplace
  • UGC NET Psychology Paper III: Exam Prep
  • Research Methods in Special Education
  • OSAT Elementary Education Subtests 1 and 2 (150 and 151) Prep
  • TECEP Abnormal Psychology: Study Guide & Test Prep
  • AEPA Elementary Education Subtest I (NT102): Practice & Study Guide

Browse by Lessons

  • What Is Borderline Personality Disorder? - Symptoms, Treatment & Therapy
  • Psychopathy: Symptoms, Definition & Treatment
  • Anger Problems & Disorders: Symptoms & Treatment
  • Drug Therapy
  • Placebo Effect | Definition, Examples & Importance
  • Support Group | Definition, Purpose & Types
  • Interpersonal Therapy | Techniques, Phases & Examples
  • Cognitive Restructuring | Definition, Therapy & Examples
  • What is Solution-Focused Brief Therapy? - Techniques & Training
  • What Are Neurological Disorders? - Symptoms, Definition & Types
  • Why Are Yawns Contagious?
  • Bullying in USA | Statistics, Facts & Prevention
  • Capgras Delusion Syndrome: Definition, Causes, & Treatment
  • Cultural Body Modification: History & Examples

Create an account to start this course today Used by over 30 million students worldwide Create an account

Explore our library of over 88,000 lessons

  • Foreign Language
  • Social Science
  • See All College Courses
  • Common Core
  • High School
  • See All High School Courses
  • College & Career Guidance Courses
  • College Placement Exams
  • Entrance Exams
  • General Test Prep
  • K-8 Courses
  • Skills Courses
  • Teacher Certification Exams
  • See All Other Courses
  • Create a Goal
  • Create custom courses
  • Get your questions answered

Module 3: Prenatal Development

Behavioral genetics, learning outcomes.

  • Describe the interaction between genetics and the environment
  • Compare monozygotic and dizygotic twins

Behavioral geneticists study how individual differences arise, in the present, through the interaction of genes and the environment. When studying human behavior, behavioral geneticists often employ twin and adoption studies to research questions of interest. Twin studies compare the rates that a given behavioral trait is shared among identical and fraternal twins; adoption studies compare those rates among biologically related relatives and adopted relatives. Both approaches provide some insight into the relative importance of genes and environment for the expression of a given trait.

Nature or Nurture?

For decades, scholars have carried on the “nature/nurture” debate. For any particular feature, those on the “nature” side would argue that heredity plays the most important role in bringing about that feature. Those on the “nurture” side would argue that one’s environment is most significant in shaping the way we are. This debate continues in questions about what makes us masculine or feminine (Lippa, 2002), concerns about vision (Mutti, Kadnik, & Adams, 1996), and many other developmental issues.

Most scholars agree that there is a constant interplay between the two forces. It is difficult to isolate the root of any single behavior as a result solely of nature or nurture, and most scholars believe that even determining the extent to which nature or nurture impacts a human feature is difficult to answer. In fact, almost all human features are polygenic (a result of many genes) and multifactorial (a result of many factors, both genetic and environmental). It is as if one’s genetic make-up sets up a range of possibilities, which may or may not be realized depending upon one’s environmental experiences. For instance, a person might be genetically predisposed to develop diabetes, but the person’s lifestyle may help bring about the disease.

When you think about your own family history, it is easy to see that there are certain personality traits, behavioral characteristics, and medical conditions that are more common than others. This is the reason that doctors ask you about your family medical history. While genetic predisposition is important to consider, there are some family members who, for a variety of reasons, seemed to defy the odds of developing these conditions. These differences can be explained in part by the effect of epigenetic (above the genome) changes.

This video explains some of the research that gives insights into the complicated relationship between nature and nurture.

You can view the transcript for “Epigenetics: Nature vs nurture” here (opens in new window) .

The Epigenetic Framework

The term “epigenetic” has been used in developmental psychology to describe psychological development as the result of an ongoing, bi-directional interchange between heredity and the environment. Gottlieb (1998; 2000; 2002) suggests an analytic framework for the nature/nurture debate that recognizes the interplay between the environment, behavior, and genetic expression. This bidirectional interplay suggests that the environment can affect the expression of genes just as genetic predispositions can impact a person’s potentials. Likewise, environmental circumstances can trigger symptoms of a genetic disorder. For example, a person predisposed genetically for type 2 diabetes may trigger the disease through poor diet and little exercise.

The developmental psychologist Erik Erikson wrote of an  epigenetic principle  in his book  Identity: Youth and Crisis  (1968), encompassing the notion that we develop through an unfolding of our personality in predetermined stages, and that our environment and surrounding culture influence how we progress through these stages. This biological unfolding in relation to our socio-cultural settings is done in stages of psychosocial development, where “progress through each stage is in part determined by our success, or lack of success, in all the previous stages.”

In typical human families, children’s biological parents raise them, so it is very difficult to know whether children act like their parents due to genetic (nature) or environmental (nurture) reasons. Nevertheless, despite our restrictions on setting up human-based experiments, we do see real-world examples of nature-nurture at work in the human sphere—though they only provide partial answers to our many questions. The science of how genes and environments work together to influence behavior is called  behavioral genetics . The easiest opportunity we have to observe this is the  adoption study . When children are put up for adoption, the parents who give birth to them are no longer the parents who raise them. Children aren’t assigned to random adoptive parents in order to suit the particular interests of a scientist but adoption still tells us some interesting things, or at least confirms some basic expectations. For instance, if the biological child of tall parents were adopted into a family of short people, do you suppose the child’s growth would be affected? What about the biological child of a Spanish-speaking family adopted at birth into an English-speaking family? What language would you expect the child to speak? And what might these outcomes tell you about the difference between height and language in terms of nature-nurture?

Monozygotic and Dizygotic Twins

Another option for observing nature-nurture in humans involves  twin studies . To analyze nature–nurture using twins, we compare the similarity of monozygotic and dizygotic pairs. Monozygotic twins occur when a single zygote or fertilized egg splits apart in the first two weeks of development. The result is the creation of two separate but genetically identical offspring. About one-third of twins are monozygotic twins. Monozygotic twins occur in birthing at a rate of about 3 in every 1000 deliveries worldwide (about 0.3% of the world population). Monozygotic twins are genetically nearly identical and they are always the same sex unless there has been a mutation during development. The children of monozygotic twins test genetically as half-siblings (or full siblings, if a pair of monozygotic twins reproduces with another pair of identical twins or with the same person), rather than first cousins.

Sometimes two eggs or ova are released and fertilized by two separate sperm. The result is  dizygotic  or fraternal twins. About two-thirds of twins are dizygotic. These two individuals share the same amount of genetic material as would any two children from the same mother and father. Older mothers are more likely to have dizygotic twins than are younger mothers and couples who use fertility drugs are also more likely to give birth to dizygotic twins. Consequently, there has been an increase in the number of fraternal twins in recent years (Bortolus et al., 1999). In vitro fertilization (IVF) techniques are more likely to create dizygotic twins. For IVF deliveries, there are nearly 21 pairs of twins for every 1,000.

In the uterus, a majority of monozygotic twins (60–70%) share the same placenta but have separate amniotic sacs. The  placenta  is a temporary organ that connects the developing fetus via the umbilical cord to the uterine wall to allow nutrient uptake, thermo-regulation, waste elimination, and gas exchange via the mother’s blood supply.  The  amniotic sac (also called the bag of waters or the membranes),  is a thin but tough transparent pair of membranes that hold a developing embryo (and later fetus) until shortly before birth. In 18–30% of monozygotic twins each fetus has a separate placenta and a separate amniotic sac. A small number (1–2%) of monozygotic twins share the same placenta and amniotic sac. Fraternal twins each have their own placenta and own amniotic sac.

Figure 1.  Monozygotic twins come from a single zygote and generally share the same placenta, although some (18-30%) have separate placentas. Dizygotic twins come from two separately fertilized eggs and have their own placentas and amniotic sacs.

Monozygotic (one egg/identical) twins can be categorized into four types depending on the timing of the separation and duplication of cells. Various types of chorionicity and amniosity (how the baby’s sac looks) in monozygotic twins are a result of when the fertilized egg divides. This is known as placentation.

Diagram showing four ways monozygotic twins are formed, depending on the timing of the separation and duplication of cells. 1: Morula with cleavage at days 1 to 3 and results in dichorionic / diamniotic formation (separate placenta, separate amnion). 2: Blastocyst with cleavage at days 4 to 8 which results in monochorionic / diamniotic formation (sharing the same placenta but having separate amnions). 3: Implanted blastocyst with cleavage at days 8 to 13 results in monochorionic / mono amniotic (shared placenta and shared amnions). 4: Formed embryonic disc with cleavage at days 13 to 15 which in this figure leads to conjoined twins (shared placenta, shared amnions, and bodies fused).

Figure 2. Various types of chorionicity and amniosity (how the baby’s sac looks) in monozygotic (one egg/identical) twins as a result of when the fertilized egg divides (Author Kevin Dufenbach)

Conjoined twins

Conjoined twins are monozygotic twins whose bodies are joined together during pregnancy. This occurs when the zygote starts to split after day 12  following fertilization and fails to separate completely. This condition occurs in about 1 in 50,000 human pregnancies. Most conjoined twins are now evaluated for surgery to attempt to separate them into separate functional bodies. The degree of difficulty rises if a vital organ or structure is shared between twins, such as the brain, heart or liver.

Vanishing twins

Researchers suspect that as many as 1 in 8 pregnancies start out as multiples, but only a single fetus is brought to full term because the other fetus has died very early in the pregnancy and has not been detected or recorded.  Early obstetric ultrasonography exams sometimes reveal an “extra” fetus, which fails to develop and instead disintegrates and vanishes in the uterus. There are several reasons for the “vanishing” fetus, including it being embodied or absorbed by the other fetus, placenta or the mother. This is known as vanishing twin syndrome. Also, in an unknown proportion of cases, two zygotes may fuse soon after fertilization, resulting in a single chimeric embryo, and, later, fetus.

Twin Studies

Using the features of height and spoken language as examples, let’s take a look at how nature and nurture apply: identical twins, unsurprisingly, are almost perfectly similar for height. The heights of fraternal twins, however, are like any other sibling pairs: more similar to each other than to people from other families, but hardly identical. This contrast between twin types gives us a clue about the role genetics plays in determining height.

Identical twins Laurent and Larry Nicolas Bourgeois, the Les Twins.

Figure 3.  Identical twins Laurent and Larry Nicolas Bourgeois, also known as the Les Twins, are internationally renowned dancers.

Now consider spoken language. If one identical twin speaks Spanish at home, the co-twin with whom she is raised almost certainly does too. But the same would be true for a pair of fraternal twins raised together. In terms of spoken language, fraternal twins are just as similar as identical twins, so it appears that the genetic match of identical twins doesn’t make much difference.

Twin and adoption studies are two instances of a much broader class of methods for observing nature-nurture called  quantitative genetics , the scientific discipline in which similarities among individuals are analyzed based on how biologically related they are. We can do these studies with siblings and half-siblings, cousins, and twins who have been separated at birth and raised separately (Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, & Segal, 1990). Such twins are very rare and play a smaller role than is commonly believed in the science of nature–nurture, or with entire extended families (Plomin, DeFries, Knopik, & Neiderhiser, 2012).

It would be satisfying to be able to say that nature–nurture studies have given us conclusive and complete evidence about where traits come from, with some traits clearly resulting from genetics and others almost entirely from environmental factors, such as child-rearing practices and personal will; but that is not the case. Instead,  everything  has turned out to have some footing in genetics. The more genetically-related people are, the more similar they are—for  everything : height, weight, intelligence, personality, mental illness, etc. Sure, it seems like common sense that some traits have a genetic bias. For example, adopted children resemble their biological parents even if they have never met them, and identical twins are more similar to each other than are fraternal twins. And while certain psychological traits, such as personality or mental illness (e.g., schizophrenia), seem reasonably influenced by genetics, it turns out that the same is true for political attitudes, how much television people watch (Plomin, Corley, DeFries, & Fulker, 1990), and whether or not they get divorced (McGue & Lykken, 1992).

  • Modification, adaptation, and original content. Authored by : Julie Lazzara for Lumen Learning. Provided by : Lumen Learning. License : CC BY: Attribution
  • Psyc 200 Lifespan Psychology. Authored by : Laura Overstreet. Located at : http://opencourselibrary.org/econ-201/ . License : Public Domain: No Known Copyright
  • The Nature Nurture Question. Provided by : Lumen Learning. Located at : https://courses.lumenlearning.com/waymaker-psychology/chapter/the-nature-nurture-question/ . License : CC BY: Attribution
  • Epigenetics. Provided by : Wikipedia. Located at : https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Epigenetics . License : CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Les Twins. Authored by : Shawn Welling. Provided by : Wikipedia. Located at : https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Les_Twins_profile.jpg . License : CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Twins. Provided by : Wikipedia. Located at : https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Twin . License : CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Epigenetics: Nature vs nurture. Authored by : Det medisinske fakultet UiO. Located at : https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=k50yMwEOWGU . License : Other . License Terms : Standard YouTube License

Encyclopedia Britannica

  • Games & Quizzes
  • History & Society
  • Science & Tech
  • Biographies
  • Animals & Nature
  • Geography & Travel
  • Arts & Culture
  • On This Day
  • One Good Fact
  • New Articles
  • Lifestyles & Social Issues
  • Philosophy & Religion
  • Politics, Law & Government
  • World History
  • Health & Medicine
  • Browse Biographies
  • Birds, Reptiles & Other Vertebrates
  • Bugs, Mollusks & Other Invertebrates
  • Environment
  • Fossils & Geologic Time
  • Entertainment & Pop Culture
  • Sports & Recreation
  • Visual Arts
  • Demystified
  • Image Galleries
  • Infographics
  • Top Questions
  • Britannica Kids
  • Saving Earth
  • Space Next 50
  • Student Center
  • Introduction

Early history

Methods of study.

Francis Galton

  • When did science begin?
  • Where was science invented?
  • Why is biology important?

Blackboard inscribed with scientific formulas and calculations in physics and mathematics

behaviour genetics

Our editors will review what you’ve submitted and determine whether to revise the article.

  • National Center for Biotechnology Information - PubMed Central - Behavioral Genetics and Child Temperament
  • Academia - Behaviour Genetics : An Overview
  • Table Of Contents

Francis Galton

Trusted Britannica articles, summarized using artificial intelligence, to provide a quicker and simpler reading experience. This is a beta feature. Please verify important information in our full article.

This summary was created from our Britannica article using AI. Please verify important information in our full article.

behaviour genetics , the study of the influence of an organism’s genetic composition on its behaviour and the interaction of heredity and environment insofar as they affect behaviour. The question of the determinants of behavioral abilities and disabilities has commonly been referred to as the “nature-nurture” controversy.

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

The relationship between behaviour and genetics , or heredity, dates to the work of English scientist Sir Francis Galton (1822–1911), who coined the phrase “nature and nurture.” Galton studied the families of outstanding men of his day and concluded, like his cousin Charles Darwin , that mental powers run in families. Galton became the first to use twins in genetic research and pioneered many of the statistical methods of analysis that are in use today. In 1918 British statistician and geneticist Ronald Aylmer Fisher published a paper that showed how Gregor Mendel ’s laws of inheritance applied to complex traits influenced by multiple genes and environmental factors.

Illustrated strands of DNA. Deoxyribonucleic acid, biology.

The first human behavioral genetic research on intelligence and mental illness began in the 1920s, when environmentalism (the theory that behaviour is a result of nongenetic factors such as various childhood experiences) became popular and before Nazi Germany’s abuse of genetics made the notion of hereditary influence abhorrent . Although genetic research on human behaviour continued throughout the following decades, it was not until the 1970s that a balanced view came to prevail in psychiatry that recognized the importance of nature as well as nurture. In psychology , this reconciliation did not take hold until the 1980s. Much behavioral genetic research today focuses on identifying specific genes that affect behavioral dimensions, such as personality and intelligence , and disorders, such as autism, hyperactivity, depression , and schizophrenia.

Quantitative genetic methods are used to estimate the net effect of genetic and environmental factors on individual differences in any complex trait , including behavioral traits. In addition, molecular genetic methods are used to identify specific genes responsible for genetic influence. Research is carried out in both animals and humans; however, studies using animal models tend to provide more-accurate data than studies in humans because both genes and environment can be manipulated and controlled in the laboratory.

By mating related animals such as siblings for many generations, nearly pure strains are obtained in which all offspring are genetically highly similar. It is possible to screen for genetic influence on behaviour by comparing the behaviour of different inbred strains raised in the same laboratory environment. Another method, known as selective breeding , evaluates genetic involvement by attempting to breed for high and low extremes of a trait for several generations. Both methods have been applied to a wide variety of animal behaviours, especially learning and behavioral responses to drugs , and this research provides evidence for widespread influence of genes on behaviour.

Because genes and environments cannot be manipulated in the human species, two quasi-experimental methods are used to screen for genetic influence on individual differences in complex traits such as behaviour. The twin method relies on the accident of nature that results in identical (monozygotic, MZ) twins or fraternal (dizygotic, DZ) twins. MZ twins are like clones , genetically identical to each other because they came from the same fertilized egg. DZ twins, on the other hand, developed from two eggs that happened to be fertilized at the same time. Like other siblings, DZ twins are only half as similar genetically as MZ twins. To the extent that behavioral variability is caused by environmental factors, DZ twins should be as similar for the behavioral trait as are MZ twins because both types of twins are reared by the same parents in the same place at the same time. If the trait is influenced by genes, then DZ twins ought to be less similar than MZ twins. For schizophrenia , for example, the concordance (risk of one twin’s being schizophrenic if the other is) is about 45 percent for MZ twins and about 15 percent for DZ twins. For intelligence as assessed by IQ tests, the correlation, an index of resemblance (0.00 indicates no resemblance and 1.00 indicates perfect resemblance), is 0.85 for MZ twins and 0.60 for DZ twins for studies throughout the world of more than 10,000 pairs of twins. The twin method has been robustly defended as a rough screen for genetic influence on behaviour.

The adoption method is a quasi-experimental design that relies on a social accident in which children are adopted away from their biological (birth) parents early in life, thus cleaving the effects of nature and nurture. Because the twin and adoption methods are so different, greater confidence is warranted when results from these two methods converge on the same conclusion—as they usually do. An influential adoption study of schizophrenia in 1966 by American behavioral geneticist Leonard Heston showed that children adopted away from their schizophrenic biological mothers at birth were just as likely to become schizophrenic (about 10 percent) as were children reared by their schizophrenic biological mothers. A 20-year study begun in the 1970s in the United States of intelligence of adopted children and their biological and adoptive parents showed increasing similarity from infancy to childhood to adolescence between the adopted children and their biological parents but no resemblance between the adopted children and their adoptive parents.

In contrast to traditional molecular genetic research that focused on rare disorders caused by a single genetic mutation , molecular genetic research on complex behavioral traits and common behavioral disorders is much more difficult because multiple genes are involved and each gene has a relatively small effect. However, some genes identified in animal models have contributed to an improved understanding of complex human behavioral disorders such as reading disability, hyperactivity, autism , and dementia .

Want to create or adapt books like this? Learn more about how Pressbooks supports open publishing practices.

4.3 Behavioral Genetics

Learning outcomes.

  • Describe the interaction between genetics and the environment
  • Compare monozygotic and dizygotic twins

Behavioral geneticists study how individual differences arise, in the present, through the interaction of genes and the environment. When studying human behavior, behavioral geneticists often employ twin and adoption studies to research questions of interest. Twin studies compare the rates that a given behavioral trait is shared among identical and fraternal twins; adoption studies compare those rates among biologically related relatives and adopted relatives. Both approaches provide some insight into the relative importance of genes and environment for the expression of a given trait.

Nature or Nurture?

For decades, scholars have carried on the “nature/nurture” debate. For any particular feature, those on the “nature” side would argue that heredity plays the most important role in bringing about that feature. Those on the “nurture” side would argue that one’s environment is most significant in shaping the way we are. This debate continues in questions about what makes us masculine or feminine (Lippa, 2002), concerns about vision (Mutti et al. 1996), and many other developmental issues.

Most scholars agree that there is a constant interplay between the two forces. It is difficult to isolate the root of any single behavior as a result solely of nature or nurture, and most scholars believe that even determining the extent to which nature or nurture impacts a human feature is difficult to answer. In fact, almost all human features are polygenic (a result of many genes) and multifactorial (a result of many factors, both genetic and environmental). It is as if one’s genetic make-up sets up a range of possibilities, which may or may not be realized depending upon one’s environmental experiences. For instance, a person might be genetically predisposed to develop diabetes, but the person’s lifestyle may help bring about the disease.

When you think about your own family history, it is easy to see that there are certain personality traits, behavioral characteristics, and medical conditions that are more common than others. This is the reason that doctors ask you about your family medical history. While genetic predisposition is important to consider, there are some family members who, for a variety of reasons, seemed to defy the odds of developing these conditions. These differences can be explained in part by the effect of epigenetic (above the genome) changes.

You can view the transcript for “Epigenetics: Nature vs nurture” here (opens in new window) .

The Epigenetic Framework

The term “epigenetic” has been used in developmental psychology to describe psychological development as the result of an ongoing, bi-directional interchange between heredity and the environment. Gottlieb (1998; 2000; 2002) suggests an analytic framework for the nature/nurture debate that recognizes the interplay between the environment, behavior, and genetic expression. This bidirectional interplay suggests that the environment can affect the expression of genes just as genetic predispositions can impact a person’s potentials. Likewise, environmental circumstances can trigger symptoms of a genetic disorder. For example, a person predisposed genetically for type 2 diabetes may trigger the disease through poor diet and little exercise.

The developmental psychologist Erik Erikson wrote of an  epigenetic principle  in his book  Identity: Youth and Crisis  (1968), encompassing the notion that we develop through an unfolding of our personality in predetermined stages, and that our environment and surrounding culture influence how we progress through these stages. This biological unfolding in relation to our socio-cultural settings is done in stages of psychosocial development, where “progress through each stage is in part determined by our success, or lack of success, in all the previous stages.”

In typical human families, children’s biological parents raise them, so it is very difficult to know whether children act like their parents due to genetic (nature) or environmental (nurture) reasons. Nevertheless, despite our restrictions on setting up human-based experiments, we do see real-world examples of nature-nurture at work in the human sphere—though they only provide partial answers to our many questions. The science of how genes and environments work together to influence behavior is called  behavioral genetics . The easiest opportunity we have to observe this is the  adoption study . When children are put up for adoption, the parents who give birth to them are no longer the parents who raise them. Children aren’t assigned to random adoptive parents in order to suit the particular interests of a scientist but adoption still tells us some interesting things, or at least confirms some basic expectations. For instance, if the biological child of tall parents were adopted into a family of short people, do you suppose the child’s growth would be affected? What about the biological child of a Spanish-speaking family adopted at birth into an English-speaking family? What language would you expect the child to speak? And what might these outcomes tell you about the difference between height and language in terms of nature-nurture?

Monozygotic and Dizygotic Twins

Another option for observing nature-nurture in humans involves  twin studies . To analyze nature–nurture using twins, we compare the similarity of monozygotic and dizygotic pairs. Monozygotic twins occur when a single zygote or fertilized egg splits apart in the first two weeks of development. The result is the creation of two separate but genetically identical offspring. About one-third of twins are monozygotic twins. Monozygotic twins occur in birthing at a rate of about 3 in every 1000 deliveries worldwide (about 0.3% of the world population). Monozygotic twins are genetically nearly identical and they are always the same sex unless there has been a mutation during development. The children of monozygotic twins test genetically as half-siblings (or full siblings, if a pair of monozygotic twins reproduces with another pair of identical twins or with the same person), rather than first cousins.

Sometimes two eggs or ova are released and fertilized by two separate sperm. The result is  dizygotic or fraternal twins. About two-thirds of twins are dizygotic. These two individuals share the same amount of genetic material as would any two children from the same two parents. Older gestational parents are more likely to have dizygotic twins than are younger gestational parents and couples who use fertility drugs are also more likely to give birth to dizygotic twins. Consequently, there has been an increase in the number of fraternal twins in recent years (Bortolus et al., 1999). In vitro fertilization (IVF) techniques are more likely to create dizygotic twins. For IVF deliveries, there are nearly 21 pairs of twins for every 1,000.

In the uterus, a majority of monozygotic twins (60–70%) share the same placenta but have separate amniotic sacs. The  placenta is a temporary organ that connects the developing fetus via the umbilical cord to the uterine wall to allow nutrient uptake, thermo-regulation, waste elimination, and gas exchange via the gestational parent’s blood supply.  The amniotic sac (also called the bag of waters or the membranes),  is a thin but tough transparent pair of membranes that hold a developing embryo (and later fetus) until shortly before birth. In 18–30% of monozygotic twins each fetus has a separate placenta and a separate amniotic sac. A small number (1–2%) of monozygotic twins share the same placenta and amniotic sac. Fraternal twins each have their own placenta and own amniotic sac.

Monozygotic (one egg/identical) twins can be categorized into four types depending on the timing of the separation and duplication of cells. Various types of chorionicity and amniosity (how the baby’s sac looks) in monozygotic twins are a result of when the fertilized egg divides. This is known as placentation.

Diagram showing four ways monozygotic twins are formed, depending on the timing of the separation and duplication of cells. 1: Morula with cleavage at days 1 to 3 and results in dichorionic / diamniotic formation (separate placenta, separate amnion). 2: Blastocyst with cleavage at days 4 to 8 which results in monochorionic / diamniotic formation (sharing the same placenta but having separate amnions). 3: Implanted blastocyst with cleavage at days 8 to 13 results in monochorionic / mono amniotic (shared placenta and shared amnions). 4: Formed embryonic disc with cleavage at days 13 to 15 which in this figure leads to conjoined twins (shared placenta, shared amnions, and bodies fused).

Conjoined twins

Conjoined twins are monozygotic twins whose bodies are joined together during pregnancy. This occurs when the zygote starts to split after day 12  following fertilization and fails to separate completely. This condition occurs in about 1 in 50,000 human pregnancies. Most conjoined twins are now evaluated for surgery to attempt to separate them into separate functional bodies. The degree of difficulty rises if a vital organ or structure is shared between twins, such as the brain, heart or liver.

Vanishing twins

Researchers suspect that as many as 1 in 8 pregnancies start out as multiples, but only a single fetus is brought to full term because the other fetus has died very early in the pregnancy and has not been detected or recorded. Early obstetric ultrasonography exams sometimes reveal an “extra” fetus, which fails to develop and instead disintegrates and vanishes in the uterus. There are several reasons for the “vanishing” fetus, including it being embodied or absorbed by the other fetus, placenta, or the gestational parent. This is known as vanishing twin syndrome. Also, in an unknown proportion of cases, two zygotes may fuse soon after fertilization, resulting in a single chimeric embryo, and, later, fetus.

Twin Studies

Using the features of height and spoken language as examples, let’s take a look at how nature and nurture apply: identical twins, unsurprisingly, are almost perfectly similar for height. The heights of fraternal twins, however, are like any other sibling pairs: more similar to each other than to people from other families, but hardly identical. This contrast between twin types gives us a clue about the role genetics plays in determining height.

Identical twins Laurent and Larry Nicolas Bourgeois, the Les Twins.

Now consider spoken language. If one identical twin speaks Spanish at home, the co-twin with whom she is raised almost certainly does too. But the same would be true for a pair of fraternal twins raised together. In terms of spoken language, fraternal twins are just as similar as identical twins, so it appears that the genetic match of identical twins doesn’t make much difference.

Twin and adoption studies are two instances of a much broader class of methods for observing nature-nurture called  quantitative genetics , the scientific discipline in which similarities among individuals are analyzed based on how biologically related they are. We can do these studies with siblings and half-siblings, cousins, and twins who have been separated at birth and raised separately (Bouchard et al., 1990). Such twins are very rare and play a smaller role than is commonly believed in the science of nature–nurture, or with entire extended families (Plomin et al., 2012).

It would be satisfying to be able to say that nature–nurture studies have given us conclusive and complete evidence about where traits come from, with some traits clearly resulting from genetics and others almost entirely from environmental factors, such as child-rearing practices and personal will; but that is not the case. Instead,  everything  has turned out to have some footing in genetics. The more genetically-related people are, the more similar they are—for  everything : height, weight, intelligence, personality, mental illness, etc. Sure, it seems like common sense that some traits have a genetic bias. For example, adopted children resemble their biological parents even if they have never met them, and identical twins are more similar to each other than are fraternal twins. And while certain psychological traits, such as personality or mental illness (e.g., schizophrenia), seem reasonably influenced by genetics, it turns out that the same is true for political attitudes, how much television people watch (Plomin et al. 1990), and whether or not they get divorced (McGue & Lykken, 1992).

Bortolus, R., Parazzini, F., Chatenoud, L., Benzi, G., Bianchi, M. M., & Marini, A. (1999). The epidemiology of multiple births. Human Reproduction Update, 5, 179-187.

Bouchard Jr, T. J., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota study of twins reared apart.  Science ,  250 (4978), 223-228.

Erikson, E. H. (1968).  Identity: Youth and Crisis  (No. 7). WW Norton & company.

Gottlieb, G. (1998). Normally occurring environmental and behavioral influences on gene activity: From central dogma to probabilistic epigenesis. Psychological Review, 105 , 792-802.

Gottlieb, G. (2000). Environmental and behavioral influences on gene activity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9 , 93-97.

Gottlieb, G. (2002). Individual development and evolution: The genesis of novel behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.

Lippa, R. A. (2002). Gender, nature, and nurture . Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum.

McGue, M., & Lykken, D. T. (1992). Genetic influence on risk of divorce. Psychological Science,  3 (6), 368-373.

Mutti, D. O., Zadnik, K., & Adams, A. J. (n.d.). Myopia. The nature versus nurture debate goes on. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science. Retrieved May 03, 2011, from  http://www.iovs.org/cgi/reprint/37/6/952

Plomin, R., Corley, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1990). Individual differences in television viewing in early childhood: Nature as well as nurture.  Psychological Science , 1(6), 371-377.

Plomin R. DeFries J. C. Knopik V. S. & Neiderhiser J. M. (2013).  Behavioral genetics  (Sixth). Worth.

CC Licensed Content

  • Behavioral Genetics. Authored by : Julie Lazzara for Lumen Learning.  Provided by : Lumen Learning.  Located at: https://courses.lumenlearning.com/wm-lifespandevelopment/chapter/behavioral-genetics/ .  License :  CC BY: Attribution
  • Psyc 200 Lifespan Psychology.  Authored by : Laura Overstreet.  Located at :  http://opencourselibrary.org/econ-201/ .  License :  Public Domain: No Known Copyright
  • The Nature Nurture Question.  Provided by : Lumen Learning.  Located at :  https://courses.lumenlearning.com/waymaker-psychology/chapter/the-nature-nurture-question/ .  License :  CC BY: Attribution
  • Epigenetics.  Provided by : Wikipedia.  Located at :  https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Epigenetics .  License :  CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike

Media Attributions

  • Les Twins.  Authored by : Shawn Welling.  Provided by : Wikipedia.  Located at :  https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:Les_Twins_profile.jpg .  License :  CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Identical-Fraternal Twins. Provided by : Wikipedia.  Located at : https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Identical-fraternal-sperm-egg.svg .  License :  CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Placentation.  Provided by : Wikipedia.  Located at : https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Placentation.svg . License :  CC BY-SA: Attribution-ShareAlike
  • Epigenetics: Nature vs nurture.  Authored by : Det medisinske fakultet UiO.  Located at :  https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=k50yMwEOWGU .  License :  Other .  License Terms : Standard YouTube License

4.3 Behavioral Genetics Copyright © by Meredith Palm is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License , except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book

Online ordering is currently unavailable due to technical issues. We apologise for any delays responding to customers while we resolve this. For further updates please visit our website: https://www.cambridge.org/news-and-insights/technical-incident Due to planned maintenance there will be periods of time where the website may be unavailable. We apologise for any inconvenience.

We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings .

Login Alert

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

  • > Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology
  • > Behavior Genetic Research Methods

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Book contents

  • Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology
  • Copyright page
  • Contributors
  • Introduction to the Second Edition
  • Introduction to the First Edition
  • Chapter one Scratch an Itch with a Brick
  • Part one Design and Inference Considerations
  • Part two Procedural Possibilities
  • Chapter six Using Physiological Indexes in Social Psychological Research
  • Chapter seven Research Methods in Social and Affective Neuroscience
  • Chapter eight Behavior Genetic Research Methods
  • Chapter nine Methods of Small Group Research
  • Chapter ten Inducing and Measuring Emotion and Affect
  • Chapter eleven Complex Dynamical Systems in Social and Personality Psychology
  • Chapter twelve Implicit Measures in Social and Personality Psychology
  • Chapter thirteen The Mind in the Middle
  • Chapter fourteen Behavioral Observation and Coding
  • Chapter fifteen Methods for Studying Everyday Experience in Its Natural Context
  • Chapter sixteen Survey Research
  • Chapter seventeen Conducting Research on the Internet
  • Part three Data Analytic Strategies
  • Author Index
  • Subject Index

Chapter eight - Behavior Genetic Research Methods

Testing Quasi-Causal Hypotheses Using Multivariate Twin Data

from Part two - Procedural Possibilities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2014

Access options

Save book to kindle.

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle .

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service .

  • Behavior Genetic Research Methods
  • By Eric Turkheimer , K. Paige Harden
  • Edited by Harry T. Reis , University of Rochester, New York , Charles M. Judd , University of Colorado Boulder
  • Book: Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology
  • Online publication: 05 June 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511996481.012

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox .

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive .

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Review Article
  • Published: 11 May 2020

Using genetics for social science

  • K. Paige Harden   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1557-6737 1 &
  • Philipp D. Koellinger   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0001-7413-0412 2  

Nature Human Behaviour volume  4 ,  pages 567–576 ( 2020 ) Cite this article

7251 Accesses

71 Citations

509 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Politics and international relations

Social science genetics is concerned with understanding whether, how and why genetic differences between human beings are linked to differences in behaviours and socioeconomic outcomes. Our review discusses the goals, methods, challenges and implications of this research endeavour. We survey how the recent developments in genetics are beginning to provide social scientists with a powerful new toolbox they can use to better understand environmental effects, and we illustrate this with several substantive examples. Furthermore, we examine how medical research can benefit from genetic insights into social-scientific outcomes and vice versa. Finally, we discuss the ethical challenges of this work and clarify several common misunderstandings and misinterpretations of genetic research on individual differences.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Access Nature and 54 other Nature Portfolio journals

Get Nature+, our best-value online-access subscription

24,99 € / 30 days

cancel any time

Subscribe to this journal

Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles

111,21 € per year

only 9,27 € per issue

Buy this article

  • Purchase on Springer Link
  • Instant access to full article PDF

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Similar content being viewed by others

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Genetic determinism, essentialism and reductionism: semantic clarity for contested science

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Moving back to the future of big data-driven research: reflecting on the social in genomics

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Beyond the factor indeterminacy problem using genome-wide association data

Data availability.

The genetic correlations reported in Fig. 1 are based on publicly available GWAS summary statistics on LDHub ( http://ldsc.broadinstitute.org/ldhub/ ). The Health and Retirement Study data in Fig. 3 can be accessed via dbGaP and the University of Michigan.

Definition of social science . Merriam Webster Dictionary https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/social%20science (Accessed 1 November 2018).

Turkheimer, E. Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 9 , 160–164 (2000).

Google Scholar  

Polderman, T. J. C. et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat. Genet. 47 , 702–709 (2015).

CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Benjamin, D. J. et al. The promises and pitfalls of genoeconomics. Annu. Rev. Econ. 4 , 627–662 (2012).

Visscher, P. M. et al. 10 years of GWAS discovery: Biology, function, and translation. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 101 , 5–22 (2017).

CAS   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Buniello, A. et al. The NHGRI-EBI GWAS Catalog of published genome-wide association studies, targeted arrays and summary statistics 2019. Nucleic Acids Res. 47 D1, D1005–D1012 (2019).

Freese, J. The arrival of social science genomics. Contemp. Sociol. 47 , 524–536 (2018).

Comfort, N. Nature still battles nurture in the haunting world of social genomics. Nature 553 , 278–280 (2018).

Turkheimer, E. & Paige Harden, K. Behavior Genetic Research Methods. in Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology 159–187 (Cambridge University Press, 2014).

Kong, A. et al. The nature of nurture: Effects of parental genotypes. Science 359 , 424–428 (2018).

Koellinger, P. D. & Harden, K. P. Using nature to understand nurture: Genetic associations show how parenting matters for children’s education. Science 359 , 386–387 (2018).

Bycroft, C. et al. The UK Biobank resource with deep phenotyping and genomic data. Nature 562 , 203–209 (2018).

Servick, K. Can 23andMe have it all? Science 349 , 1472–1474, 1476–1477 (2015).

Duncan, L. E., Pollastri, A. R. & Smoller, J. W. Mind the gap: why many geneticists and psychological scientists have discrepant views about gene-environment interaction (G×E) research. Am. Psychol. 69 , 249–268 (2014).

PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Reich, D. E. et al. Linkage disequilibrium in the human genome. Nature 411 , 199–204 (2001).

Abecasis, G. R. et al. The 1000 Genomes Project Consortium. An integrated map of genetic variation from 1,092 human genomes. Nature 491 , 56–65 (2012).

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Price, A. L. et al. Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nat. Genet. 38 , 904–909 (2006).

Visscher, P. M., Brown, M. A., McCarthy, M. I. & Yang, J. Five years of GWAS discovery. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 90 , 7–24 (2012).

Chabris, C. F., Lee, J. J., Cesarini, D., Benjamin, D. J. & Laibson, D. I. The fourth law of behavior genetics. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24 , 304–312 (2015).

Rapid GWAS of thousands of phenotypes for 337,000 samples in the UK Biobank. Neale Lab http://www.nealelab.is/blog/2017/7/19/rapid-gwas-of-thousands-of-phenotypes-for-337000-samples-in-the-uk-biobank (Accessed 6 November 2018).

Kyoko Watanabe, D.P. Atlas of GWAS Summary Statistics. GWAS Atlas ( 2017 ) . http://atlas.ctglab.nl/ (Accessed 6 November 2019).

Global Biobank Engine. https://biobankengine.stanford.edu/ (Accessed 6 November 2019).

Manolio, T. A. et al. Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature 461 , 747–753 (2009).

Sohail, M. et al. Polygenic adaptation on height is overestimated due to uncorrected stratification in genome-wide association studies. eLife 8 , e39702 (2019).

Berg, J. J. et al. Reduced signal for polygenic adaptation of height in UK Biobank. eLife 8 , e39725 (2019).

Haworth, S. et al. Apparent latent structure within the UK Biobank sample has implications for epidemiological analysis. Nat. Commun. 10 , 333 (2019).

Lawson, D. J. et al. Is population structure in the genetic biobank era irrelevant, a challenge, or an opportunity? Hum. Genet. 139 , 23–41 (2020).

Loh, P.-R. et al. Efficient Bayesian mixed-model analysis increases association power in large cohorts. Nat. Genet. 47 , 284–290 (2015).

Yang, J., Zaitlen, N. A., Goddard, M. E., Visscher, P. M. & Price, A. L. Advantages and pitfalls in the application of mixed-model association methods. Nat. Genet. 46 , 100–106 (2014).

Davies, N. M. et al. Within family Mendelian randomization studies. Hum. Mol. Genet. 28 R2 R170–R179, https://doi.org/10.1093/hmg/ddz204 (2019).

Bulik-Sullivan, B. et al. An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits. Nat. Genet. 47 , 1236–1241 (2015).

Lee, J. J., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G. & Chow, C. C. The accuracy of LD Score regression as an estimator of confounding and genetic correlations in genome-wide association studies. Genet. Epidemiol. 42 , 783–795 (2018).

Zheng, J. et al. LD Hub: a centralized database and web interface to perform LD score regression that maximizes the potential of summary level GWAS data for SNP heritability and genetic correlation analysis. Bioinformatics 33 , 272–279 (2017).

Lee, J. J. et al. Gene discovery and polygenic prediction from a genome-wide association study of educational attainment in 1.1 million individuals. Nat. Genet. 50 , 1112–1121 (2018).

Gage, S. H., Davey Smith, G., Ware, J. J., Flint, J. & Munafò, M. R. G = E: what GWAS can tell us about the environment. PLoS Genet. 12 , e1005765 (2016).

Turley, P. et al. Multi-trait analysis of genome-wide association summary statistics using MTAG. Nat. Genet. 50 , 229–237 (2018).

Grotzinger, A. D. et al. Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3 , 513–525 (2019).

Baselmans, B. M. L. et al. Multivariate genome-wide analyses of the well-being spectrum. Nat. Genet. 51 , 445–451 (2019).

Loehlin, J. C. The Cholesky approach: A cautionary note. Behav. Genet. 26 , 65–69 (1996).

Torkamani, A., Wineinger, N. E. & Topol, E. J. The personal and clinical utility of polygenic risk scores. Nat. Rev. Genet. 19 , 581–590 (2018).

Dudbridge, F. Power and predictive accuracy of polygenic risk scores. PLoS Genet. 9 , e1003348 (2013).

Wray, N. R. et al. Research review: Polygenic methods and their application to psychiatric traits. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55 , 1068–1087 (2014).

de Vlaming, R. et al. Meta-GWAS accuracy and power (MetaGAP) calculator shows that hiding heritability is partially due to imperfect genetic correlations across studies. PLoS Genet. 13 , e1006495 (2017).

Rietveld, C. A. et al. GWAS of 126,559 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with educational attainment. Science 340 , 1467–1471 (2013).

Okbay, A. et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 74 loci associated with educational attainment. Nature 533 , 539–542 (2016).

Wood, A. R. et al. Defining the role of common variation in the genomic and biological architecture of adult human height. Nat. Genet. 46 , 1173–1186 (2014).

Yengo, L. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for height and body mass index in ∼ 700000 individuals of European ancestry. Hum. Mol. Genet. 27 , 3641–3649 (2018).

Belsky, D. W. & Harden, K. P. Phenotypic annotation: Using polygenic scores to translate discoveries from genome-wide association studies from the top down. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 28 , 82–90 (2019).

Barcellos, S. H., Carvalho, L. S. & Turley, P. Education can reduce health differences related to genetic risk of obesity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115 , E9765–E9772 (2018).

Bansal, V. et al. Genome-wide association study results for educational attainment aid in identifying genetic heterogeneity of schizophrenia. Nat. Commun. 9 , 3078 (2018).

Belsky, D. W. et al. The genetics of success: how single-nucleotide polymorphisms associated with educational attainment relate to life-course development. Psychol. Sci. 27 , 957–972 (2016).

Young, A. I., Benonisdottir, S., Przeworski, M. & Kong, A. Deconstructing the sources of genotype-phenotype associations in humans. Science 365 , 1396–1400 (2019).

Mostafavi, H., Harpak, A., Conley, D., Pritchard, J. K. & Przeworski, M. Variable prediction accuracy of polygenic scores within an ancestry group. eLife 9 , e48376 (2020).

DiPrete, T. A., Burik, C. A. P. & Koellinger, P. D. Genetic instrumental variable regression: Explaining socioeconomic and health outcomes in nonexperimental data. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115 , E4970–E4979 (2018).

Wertz, J. et al. Genetics of nurture: A test of the hypothesis that parents’ genetics predict their observed caregiving. Dev. Psychol. 55 , 1461–1472 (2019).

SSGAC Polygenic Score Data. Health and Retirement Study https://hrs.isr.umich.edu/news/ssgac-polygenic-score-data (Accessed 11 November 2019).

Young, A. I. et al. Relatedness disequilibrium regression estimates heritability without environmental bias. Nat. Genet. 50 , 1304–1310 (2018).

Davey Smith, G. & Hemani, G. Mendelian randomization: genetic anchors for causal inference in epidemiological studies. Hum. Mol. Genet. 23 R1 R89–R98 (2014).

Brumpton, B. et al. Within-family studies for Mendelian randomization: avoiding dynastic, assortative mating, and population stratification biases. bioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/602516 (2019).

O’Connor, L. J. & Price, A. L. Distinguishing genetic correlation from causation across 52 diseases and complex traits. Nat. Genet. 50 , 1728–1734 (2018).

Zhu, Z. et al. Causal associations between risk factors and common diseases inferred from GWAS summary data. Nat. Commun. 9 , 224 (2018).

Verbanck, M., Chen, C.-Y., Neale, B. & Do, R. Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases. Nat. Genet. 50 , 693–698 (2018).

Koellinger, P. D. & de Vlaming, R. Mendelian randomization: the challenge of unobserved environmental confounds. Int. J. Epidemiol. 48 , 665–671 (2019).

Black, S. E., Devereux, P. J. & Salvanes, K. G. Why the apple doesn’t fall far: Understanding intergenerational transmission of human capital. Am. Econ. Rev. 95 , 437–449 (2005).

Bates, T. C. et al. The nature of nurture: Using a virtual-parent design to test parenting effects on children’s educational attainment in genotyped families. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 21 , 73–83 (2018).

Liu, H. Social and genetic pathways in multigenerational transmission of educational attainment. Am. Sociol. Rev. 83 , 278–304 (2018).

Belsky, D. W. et al. Genetic analysis of social-class mobility in five longitudinal studies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115 , E7275–E7284 (2018).

Barth, D., Papageorge, N. W. & Thom, K. Genetic endowments and wealth inequality. J. Polit. Econ. 128 , 1474–1522 (2020).

Holland, P. W. Statistics and causal inference. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 81 , 945–960 (1986).

Rodgers, J. & Kohler, H.-P. The Biodemography of Human Reproduction and Fertility . (Springer, 2002).

Barban, N. et al. Genome-wide analysis identifies 12 loci influencing human reproductive behavior. Nat. Genet. 48 , 1462–1472 (2016).

Day, F. R. et al. Genomic analyses identify hundreds of variants associated with age at menarche and support a role for puberty timing in cancer risk. Nat. Genet. 49 , 834–841 (2017).

Day, F. R. et al. Physical and neurobehavioral determinants of reproductive onset and success. Nat. Genet. 48 , 617–623 (2016).

Mehta, D. et al. Evidence for genetic overlap between schizophrenia and age at first birth in women. JAMA Psychiatry 73 , 497–505 (2016).

Ni, G., Gratten, J., Wray, N. R. & Lee, S. H., Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Age at first birth in women is genetically associated with increased risk of schizophrenia. Sci. Rep. 8 , 10168 (2018).

Harden, K. P. et al. A behavior genetic investigation of adolescent motherhood and offspring mental health problems. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 116 , 667–683 (2007).

Beauchamp, J. P. Genetic evidence for natural selection in humans in the contemporary United States. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113 , 7774–7779 (2016).

Kong, A. et al. Selection against variants in the genome associated with educational attainment. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 114 , E727–E732 (2017).

Rodgers, J. L. et al. Education and cognitive ability as direct, mediating, or spurious influences on female age at first birth: behavior genetic models fit to Danish twin data. Am. J.Sociol. 114 , S202–S232 (2008). Suppl.

Tropf, F. C. & Mandemakers, J. J. Is the association between education and fertility postponement causal? The role of family background factors. Demography 54 , 71–91 (2017).

Jocklin, V., McGue, M. & Lykken, D. T. Personality and divorce: a genetic analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 71 , 288–299 (1996).

D’Onofrio, B. M., Eaves, L. J., Murrelle, L., Maes, H. H. & Spilka, B. Understanding biological and social influences on religious affiliation, attitudes, and behaviors: a behavior genetic perspective. J. Pers. 67 , 953–984 (1999).

Pilling, L. C. et al. Human longevity: 25 genetic loci associated in 389,166 UK biobank participants. Aging (Albany NY) 9 , 2504–2520 (2017).

CAS   Google Scholar  

Abdellaoui, A. et al. Genetic correlates of social stratification in Great Britain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3 , 1332–1342 (2019).

Hill, W. D. et al. Molecular genetic contributions to social deprivation and household income in UK Biobank. Curr. Biol. 26 , 3083–3089 (2016).

Belsky, D. W. et al. Genetics and the geography of health, behaviour and attainment. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3 , 576–586 (2019).

van der Sluis, S., Posthuma, D. & Dolan, C. V. A note on false positives and power in G × E modelling of twin data. Behav. Genet. 42 , 170–186 (2012).

Duncan, L. E. & Keller, M. C. A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am. J. Psychiatry 168 , 1041–1049 (2011).

Ceci, S. J. & Papierno, P. B. The rhetoric and reality of gap closing: when the “have-nots” gain but the “haves” gain even more. Am. Psychol. 60 , 149–160 (2005).

Fletcher, J. M. Why have tobacco control policies stalled? Using genetic moderation to examine policy impacts. PLoS One 7 , e50576 (2012).

Goldberger, A. S. Heritability. Economica 46 , 327–347 (1979).

Jencks, C. Heredity, environment, and public policy reconsidered. Am. Sociol. Rev. 45 , 723–736 (1980).

Bronfenbrenner, U. & Ceci, S. J. Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: a bioecological model. Psychol. Rev. 101 , 568–586 (1994).

Dickens, W. T. & Flynn, J. R. Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: the IQ paradox resolved. Psychol. Rev. 108 , 346–369 (2001).

Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., McGue, M. & Tellegen, A. Heritability of interests: a twin study. J. Appl. Psychol. 78 , 649–661 (1993).

Tucker-Drob, E. M. & Harden, K. P. Early childhood cognitive development and parental cognitive stimulation: evidence for reciprocal gene-environment transactions. Dev. Sci. 15 , 250–259 (2012).

Klahr, A. M., Thomas, K. M., Hopwood, C. J., Klump, K. L. & Burt, S. A. Evocative gene-environment correlation in the mother-child relationship: a twin study of interpersonal processes. Dev. Psychopathol. 25 , 105–118 (2013).

Scarr, S. & McCartney, K. How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype greater than environment effects. Child Dev. 54 , 424–435 (1983).

Wray, N. R. et al. Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat. Genet. 50 , 668–681 (2018).

Wehby, G. L., Domingue, B. W. & Wolinsky, F. D. Genetic risks for chronic conditions: Implications for long-term wellbeing. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 73 , 477–483 (2018).

Mallard, T. T., Harden, K. P. & Fromme, K. Genetic risk for schizophrenia is associated with substance use in emerging adulthood: an event-level polygenic prediction model. Psychol. Med. 49 , 2027–2035 (2019).

Okbay, A. et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat. Genet. 48 , 624–633 (2016).

Wootton, R. E. et al. Evaluation of the causal effects between subjective wellbeing and cardiometabolic health: mendelian randomisation study. Br. Med. J. 362 , k3788 (2018).

Kevles, D.J. In the Name of Eugenics: Genetics and the Uses of Human Heredity . (Harvard University Press, 1995).

Rawls, J. A Theory of Justice . (Belknap Press, 1999).

Herrnstein, R.J. & Murray, C. The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life (Free Press, 1996).

Jensen, A. How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement. Harv. Educ. Rev. 39 , 1–123 (1969).

Yudell, M., Roberts, D., DeSalle, R. & Tishkoff, S. Taking race out of human genetics. Science 351 , 564–565 (2016).

Smedley, A. & Smedley, B. D. Race as biology is fiction, racism as a social problem is real: Anthropological and historical perspectives on the social construction of race. Am. Psychol. 60 , 16–26 (2005).

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J. & Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33 , 61–83 (2010). discussion 83–135.

Martin, A. R. et al. Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nat. Genet. 51 , 584–591 (2019).

Martin, A. R. et al. Human demographic history impacts genetic risk prediction across diverse populations. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 100 , 635–649 (2017).

Panofsky, A. & Donovan, J. Genetic ancestry testing among white nationalists: From identity repair to citizen science. Soc. Stud. Sci . https://doi.org/10.1177/0306312719861434 (2019).

Novembre, J. & Barton, N. H. Tread lightly interpreting polygenic tests of selection. Genetics 208 , 1351–1355 (2018).

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank C. Burik for preparing Fig. 1 and the Social Science Genetic Association Consortium ( https://www.thessgac.org/ ) for Fig. 3 . P.D.K. was financially supported by an ERC consolidator grant (647648 EdGe). K.P.H. was supported by the Jacobs Foundation, the Templeton Foundation and Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) grants R01-HD083613 and 5-R24-HD042849 (to the Population Research Center at the University of Texas at Austin). The funders had no role in the conceptualization, preparation or decision to publish this work.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, Texas, USA

K. Paige Harden

Department of Economics, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Philipp D. Koellinger

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to K. Paige Harden or Philipp D. Koellinger .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Editor recognition statement: Primary handling editor: Stavroula Kousta.

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Harden, K.P., Koellinger, P.D. Using genetics for social science. Nat Hum Behav 4 , 567–576 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0862-5

Download citation

Received : 31 January 2019

Accepted : 16 March 2020

Published : 11 May 2020

Issue Date : 01 June 2020

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0862-5

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

This article is cited by

Infrastructuring educational genomics: associations, architectures, and apparatuses.

  • Ben Williamson
  • Dimitra Kotouza
  • Jessica Pykett

Postdigital Science and Education (2024)

Could interventions on physical activity mitigate genomic liability for obesity? Applying the health disparity framework in genetically informed studies

  • Moritz Herle
  • Andrew Pickles
  • Bianca L. De Stavola

European Journal of Epidemiology (2023)

The impact of digital media on children’s intelligence while controlling for genetic differences in cognition and socioeconomic background

  • Bruno Sauce
  • Magnus Liebherr
  • Torkel Klingberg

Scientific Reports (2022)

Delayed tracking and inequality of opportunity: Gene-environment interactions in educational attainment

  • Antonie Knigge
  • Dorret I. Boomsma

npj Science of Learning (2022)

Genetics of cognitive performance, education and learning: from research to policy?

  • Peter M. Visscher

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

American Psychological Association Logo

This page has been archived and is no longer being updated regularly.

Cover Story

A second look at twin studies

As behavioral genetics enters a second century, the field's oldest research method remains both relevant and controversial.

By LEA WINERMAN

Monitor Staff

April 2004, Vol 35, No. 4

Print version: page 46

"Twins have a special claim upon our attention; it is, that their history affords means of distinguishing between the effects of tendencies received at birth, and those that were imposed by the special circumstances of their after lives."

-- Sir Francis Galton, 19th century behavioral genetics pioneer, Inquiries into Human Faculty and its Development, 1875

More than a century after Galton's observation, twin studies remain a favorite tool of behavioral geneticists. Researchers have used twin studies to try to disentangle the environmental and genetic backgrounds of a cornucopia of traits, from aggression to intelligence to schizophrenia to alcohol dependence.

But despite the popularity of twin studies, some psychologists have long questioned assumptions that underlie them--like the supposition that fraternal and identical twins share equal environments or that people choose mates with traits unlike their own. The equal environments assumption, for example, has been debated for at least 40 years. Many researchers have found evidence that the assumption is valid, but others remain skeptical (see Further Reading below).

Overall, twin studies assumptions remain controversial, says psychologist James Jaccard, PhD, a psychologist who studies statistical methods at the University at Albany of the State University of New York. In response, though, researchers are working to expand and develop twin study designs and statistical methods. And while the assumptions question remains a stumbling block for some researchers, many agree twin studies will continue to be an important tool--along with emerging genome and molecular research methods (see article page 42 )--in shedding light on human behavioral genetics.

Methods and theory

The classical twin study design relies on studying twins raised in the same family environments. Monozygotic (identical) twins share all of their genes, while dizygotic (fraternal) twins share only about 50 percent of them. So, if a researcher compares the similarity between sets of identical twins to the similarity between sets of fraternal twins for a particular trait, then any excess likeness between the identical twins should be due to genes rather than environment.

Researchers use this method, and variations on it, to estimate the heritability of traits: The percentage of variance in a population due to genes. Modern twin studies also try to quantify the effect of a person's shared environment (family) and unique environment (the individual events that shape a life) on a trait.

The assumptions those studies rest on--questioned by some psychologists, including, in recent work, Jaccard--include:

Random mating. Twin researchers assume that people are as likely to choose partners who are different from themselves as they are to choose partners who are similar for a particular trait. If, instead, people tend to choose mates like themselves, then fraternal twins could share more than 50 percent of their genes--and hence more similarities on genetically influenced traits--because they would receive similar genes from their mothers and fathers.

Equal environments. Twin researchers also assume that fraternal and identical twins raised in the same homes experience equally similar environments. But some research suggests that parents, teachers, peers and others may treat identical twins more similarly than fraternal twins.

Gene-environment interaction. Some researchers think that interactions between genes and environment, rather than genes and environment separately, may influence many traits. A recent study from Science (Vol. 297, No. 5582) by Avshalom Caspi, PhD, of King's College London, for example, suggests that a gene might moderate propensity for violence, particularly in people who are severely maltreated as children. Many twin study designs don't take this type of complication into account.

Genetic mechanisms. Traits can be inherited through different genetic mechanisms. For traits governed by dominant genetic mechanisms, a dominant gene inherited from one parent trumps a recessive gene inherited from the other parent: If a person inherits a recessive gene for blue eyes from one parent and a dominant gene for brown eyes from the other parent, then the dominant brown gene wins, and the person's eyes are brown.

Additive genetic mechanisms, in contrast, mix together--a plant that receives one red gene and one white gene might, if the genes are additive, turn out pink. Epistatic mechanisms are complex cases where interactions among multiple genes may determine the outcome of one trait. Twin studies, in general, assume that only one type of genetic mechanism--usually additive--is operating for a particular trait.

The value of twin studies

Twin researchers acknowledge that these and other limitations exist. But, they say, the limitations don't negate the usefulness of twin studies. For traits that are substantially influenced by heredity, the approximately two-fold difference in genetic similarity between the two types of twins should outweigh any complications, says John Hewitt, PhD, director of the Institute for Behavioral Genetics at the University of Colorado at Boulder.

And the extent to which different assumptions matter may depend on which trait is being studied. Studies have suggested, for example, that people are more likely to select mates with similar levels of intelligence than they are mates with similar levels of neuroticism, extraversion and other personality traits (see page 50 ). So, researchers who use twins to study intelligence might have to worry more about nonrandom mating than researchers who study personality.

Twin study designs and statistical analysis methods are also constantly evolving and improving. The original twin study design has expanded to include studies of twins' extended families, longitudinal studies and other variations. Some of these variations allow researchers to address previous limitations--they can investigate the effects of nonrandom mating, for example, by including the spouses of twins in studies. In fact, says psychologist Dorret Boomsma, PhD, of Vrije Universiteit in the Netherlands, all of these assumptions can be tested, given the proper data. She argues that they should not be seen as assumptions at all, but instead as mechanisms whose relevance can be tested using study designs that go beyond the classical twin study design.

Analysis methods, likewise, don't remain static. "People are always thinking about ways to improve the analyses," Hewitt says.

Jaccard acknowledges that this is true. "For some designs, we don't have to make as strong assumptions as we used to make," he says. "Instead of having to assume away four constructs, we only have to assume away two or three."

In the age of molecular genetics, meanwhile, the classical twin study design is only one aspect of genetics research. Twin studies estimate the heritability of a trait, but molecular genetics attempts to pinpoint the effects of a particular gene.

The future of twin research will involve combining traditional twin studies with molecular genetics research, according to Hewitt, who believes that day is already here.

"When we conduct a study of twins these days, we always get DNA on everyone," Hewitt says. "And we'll use that DNA to try and identify specific individual genes that contribute to the overall pattern of heritability."

Further Reading

Boomsma, D., Busjahn, A., & Peltonen, L. (2002). Classical twin studies and beyond. Nature Reviews Genetics, 3 (11), 872-882.

Kendler, K.S., Neale, M.C., Kessler, R.C., Heath, A.C., & Eaves, L.J. (1993). A test of the equal-environment assumption in twin studies of psychiatric illness. Behavior Genetics, 23 , 21-28.

Neale, M.C., & Cardon, L.R. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press.

Pam, A., Kemker, S.S., Ross, C.A., & Golden, R. (1996). The "equal environments assumption" in MZ-DZ twin comparisons: An untenable premise of psychiatric genetics? Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 45 (3), 349-360.

Letters to the Editor

  • Send us a letter

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • My Bibliography
  • Collections
  • Citation manager

Save citation to file

Email citation, add to collections.

  • Create a new collection
  • Add to an existing collection

Add to My Bibliography

Your saved search, create a file for external citation management software, your rss feed.

  • Search in PubMed
  • Search in NLM Catalog
  • Add to Search

Behavioral genetic methods

  • PMID: 3712205
  • DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1986.tb00394.x

Traditional behavioral genetic methods involve the use primarily of family, twin, and adoption correlations to estimate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental influences in the etiology of individual differences. These methods and representative results for personality are described. However, newer methods are emphasized: structural models and model-fitting, multivariate analysis, genetic change and continuity in development, shared and non-shared components of environmental variance, and genetic components of "environmental" variation. Because most applications of these behavioral genetic methods to the study of personality involve self-report omnibus questionnaires, an important direction for future research in this area is to use these methods to explore new issues and new measures that have emerged from personality theory and research during the past decade.

PubMed Disclaimer

Similar articles

  • A critical review of recent adoption, twin, and family studies of schizophrenia: behavioral genetics perspectives. Gottesman II, Shields J. Gottesman II, et al. Schizophr Bull. 1976;2(3):360-401. doi: 10.1093/schbul/2.3.360. Schizophr Bull. 1976. PMID: 1034336 No abstract available.
  • Shared environmental influences on personality: a combined twin and adoption approach. Matteson LK, McGue M, Iacono WG. Matteson LK, et al. Behav Genet. 2013 Nov;43(6):491-504. doi: 10.1007/s10519-013-9616-8. Epub 2013 Sep 25. Behav Genet. 2013. PMID: 24065564 Free PMC article.
  • Genetic and environmental architecture of human aggression. Miles DR, Carey G. Miles DR, et al. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997 Jan;72(1):207-17. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.72.1.207. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1997. PMID: 9008382
  • Behavioral and molecular genetic contributions to a dimensional classification of personality disorder. Livesley WJ. Livesley WJ. J Pers Disord. 2005 Apr;19(2):131-55. doi: 10.1521/pedi.19.2.131.62631. J Pers Disord. 2005. PMID: 15899713 Review.
  • The contributions of behavioural genetic studies to attachment theory. O'Connor TG, Croft C, Steele H. O'Connor TG, et al. Attach Hum Dev. 2000 Apr;2(1):107-22. doi: 10.1080/146167300361345. Attach Hum Dev. 2000. PMID: 11707890 Review.

Publication types

  • Search in MeSH

Grants and funding

  • HD-10333/HD/NICHD NIH HHS/United States
  • HD-18426/HD/NICHD NIH HHS/United States

LinkOut - more resources

Full text sources, miscellaneous.

  • NCI CPTAC Assay Portal

full text provider logo

  • Citation Manager

NCBI Literature Resources

MeSH PMC Bookshelf Disclaimer

The PubMed wordmark and PubMed logo are registered trademarks of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (HHS). Unauthorized use of these marks is strictly prohibited.

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts

Logo of nihpa

THE BIOLOGY OF RELATIONSHIPS: WHAT BEHAVIORAL GENETICS TELLS US ABOUT INTERACTIONS AMONG FAMILY MEMBERS

Laura a. baker.

* Department of Psychology, University of Southern California

Introduction

Human behavior is subject to genetic variations. The ways in which individuals differ in their intellectual abilities, personalities, and mental health are, to a large extent, functions of their inherited genetic predispositions. Decades of research on twins, adoptees, and families have led to the inescapable conclusion that most reliably measured psychological characteristics are influenced to some degree by genes. Behavior also shows signs of genetic influence; the way one experiences stressful life events, for example, shows some genetic influence. Even personal aspects of individuals, such as spirituality and political ideology, are affected to an extent by genes. 1 It should come as no surprise, then, that genes influence the ways in which families function and how family members relate to one another. 2 Familial relationships of all kinds—parent-child, sibling, and spousal—can be shown to be at least partially the product of genetic factors.

This Article discusses a behavioral genetic perspective that provides insight into the biological factors that influence family relationships. Part II presents a brief overview of the research methods used to understand both genetic and environmental influences on human behavior. Part III then discusses several key findings from the field of behavioral sciences, particularly how they pertain to the ways in which family members relate to one another. It focuses on the following: (1) characteristics of parents and variations in the ways they treat their own children; (2) characteristics of children and how they may react to their parents’ behavior; and (3) the interactive processes that occur between parents and children. While the primary focus of Part III is on parent-child relationships, Part IV considers sibling and spousal relationships. Part V discusses the general interpretation of family relationships from a behavioral genetic perspective.

II. Twin, Family, and Adoption Studies

What is the evidence for the overwhelming influence of genetic factors on human psychological function? What does it mean to say that psychological dimensions of family relations are a function of genes? Answering these questions requires a basic understanding of behavioral genetic studies, which help to separate the effects of genes and environment in human behavior.

The general strategy in behavioral-genetic research designs involves the study of family members with varying degrees of genetic and environmental relatedness. 3 For example, genetic influences in a trait are evident if pairs of monozygotic (MZ) twins (who are genetically identical) are more similar to one another than dizygotic (DZ) twins (who share only about 50% of their genes), or if pairs of biological siblings raised together resemble one another more than unrelated (e.g., adoptive) siblings raised together. In general, if psychological traits and observed behavior have a genetic component, then genetically similar relatives should resemble one another more closely than individuals who share fewer genes. 4

Regarding environmental influences, researchers in behavioral genetics typically distinguish between two broad classes of effects: (1) environmental factors shared by relatives that cause them to behave similarly; and (2) unique, individual environments that are not shared by relatives, which cause them to be different from one another. These are referred to, respectively, as shared and nonshared environments. Shared environmental influences are evident when greater trait similarity is observed for those relatives who share more experiences (e.g., siblings raised together rather than apart), or when twins are more similar to one another than their genetic relatedness would predict. Evidence for nonshared environment often stems from differences observed between genetic relatives—that is, their lack of resemblance. Differences between MZ co-twins, for example, must stem from nonshared environments. The study of the similarities and differences between relatives of varying degrees of genetic and environmental relatedness provide the basic data for understanding the effects of genes, and thus the influence of shared and nonshared environments on behavior. 5

Within a few decades of the earliest twin, family, and adoption studies (which grew immensely from the 1970s onward), genetic factors were implicated in a wide range of human behaviors, such as cognitive ability and personality, as well as most major psychological disorders, such as depression and schizophrenia. 6 Collectively, these studies show that family members who are more closely related genetically demonstrate greater similarity than unrelated individuals for measured aspects of personality (e.g., extraversion or neuroticism), intellectual function (e.g., verbal skills and spatial ability), and likelihood of being diagnosed with a psychological disorder (e.g., depression or schizophrenia). 7

For a while, it was considered a challenge to find an enduring aspect of behavior that did not appear to be influenced by genes. Constructs such as religious behaviors and political attitudes, which had traditionally been understood to be strictly the product of culture, became the subject of behavioral genetic studies. Somewhat surprisingly, even these culturally defined behaviors appear to be influenced by genetic variations, at least within groups of individuals. For example, although one’s religion may be culturally defined and thus independent of genetic influences, the degree to which one engages in the rituals or adheres to the tenets of a particular religion appear to be affected by one’s genetic inheritance. 8 Indeed, even the degree to which an individual may endorse highly liberal or conservative ideals (e.g., abortion rights or gay rights) has been shown to be influenced by genetic factors; MZ twins are much more similar than DZ twins, and biological siblings are more similar than adoptive siblings in conservative attitudes from adolescence onward. 9

Around the same time these culturally defined behaviors became the subject of behavioral genetic research, investigators began to study other variables that were traditionally viewed as entirely “environmental” factors. This research challenged a long-standing social learning perspective in developmental psychology. What were traditionally considered to be “environmental” measures—including aspects of parenting—came to be understood as products of both genes and environment. Thus, we turn back now to the issue at hand: the various aspects of family relationships and how they are influenced by the complex interplay between genes and environment.

III. How the Behavior of Parents and children Is Influenced by Genes and Environment

Genes influence each individual’s behavioral and psychological characteristics, including intellectual ability, personality, and risk for mental illness—all of which have bearing on both parents and children within a family. The ways in which genes and environment can affect parent-child relationships can be seen in Figure 1 . This model represents a standard way in which behavioral geneticists think about human behavior in the context of family relationships. Parents’ genes influence their own behavior (including the ways they parent their children) and children’s genes influence their own behavior (including the ways they respond to their parents). The transmission of genes from parent to child is one important link that will lead to similarities between the behavior of a parent and a child. For example, to the extent that genes predispose an individual toward aggressive behavior, including violence toward others, parents and children will show similarities in this area of behavior. This might offer another explanation for the “cycle of violence” in which abusive parenting is related to aggression and other antisocial behaviors in children. 10 Antisocial behavior does, in fact, show moderate genetic influence in a wide range of studies. 11

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms741938f1.jpg

Behavioral Genetic Model of Parent-Child Behavior

Besides direct genetic transmission, the model in Figure 1 indicates two other important ways in which the behavior of parents and children may be linked. First, parental behavior may itself be an important aspect of the child’s environment, which may be considered a form of “cultural transmission.” For example, a mother’s intelligence, personality, and mental health may have an impact on the child’s environment; mothers with higher intelligence and education spend more time reading to their children and engaging them in stimulating activities. 12 Importantly, however, these characteristics may each be influenced by the mother’s genetic makeup, and thus it can be seen how the mother’s genotype may ultimately be associated with the child’s environment. The association between genes and environment is generally referred to as a genotype-environment correlation (r GE ). One way in which r GE may arise is through this passive form of cultural transmission, which is referred to as a passive r GE . 13

The third link between the behavior of parents and children is established through the “evocative responses” that children’s behavior may elicit from their parents. Because a child’s behavior is itself influenced by the child’s genes, genetically different children living in the same family may elicit different parenting responses. This may result in another form of a genotype-environment correlation, an r GE of an evocative form. 14 That is, genetically based differences among children (e.g., temperament characteristics) may evoke different responses from their parents (e.g., disciplinary styles). Thus, genes and environment may be intertwined in complex ways within parent-child relationships.

These complexities can be unraveled by twin, family, and adoption studies. Genetic influences on parenting behavior can be understood by examining the similarities and differences in adult twins’ parenting styles. The parenting styles of adult twins—as measured by positivity, negativity, and monitoring of their children—were more highly correlated for MZ than DZ twins. 15 Reviews of other studies show similar patterns, in which parents’ genes influence the ways in which they parent their children. 16

Evidence of parental behaviors evoked by children has been demonstrated by studying how parents respond differently to two or more children in the same family, such as twins and other siblings. DZ twin children, for example, have reported more differences than MZ twins in levels of affect and warmth received by their parents, 17 a finding that has been replicated by using reports from parents about their own behavior, as well as by observing parents interacting with their different children. 18 Studies of adopted children have also revealed evocative responses in the rearing parents as a function of the child’s genetic predispositions, as measured by characteristics in their birth parents. More coercive parenting and negative affect were reported by the adoptive parents of children born to more antisocial parents. 19 These genetically high risk children displayed more conduct problems as children and adolescents, 20 and thus may have elicited more negative parenting. The key point is that the direction of causality may not necessarily run from parent to child; when children elicit parental behaviors, it can move in the reverse direction.

Passive r GE effects are best understood in studies comparing parent-child relationships in adopted and nonadopted children. Since adopted children are not genetically related to their rearing parents, the passive r GE does not influence their similarity, because the parents’ genes are not linked to the children’s environments. If passive r GE effects arise, whether through cultural transmission effects or other mechanisms, 21 correlations between parenting characteristics and child outcomes should be stronger when parents are raising their own genetic children. In fact, one study of adoptive and nonadoptive families found that parents’ ratings of family cohesion, low conflict, and open communication about feelings in early childhood were associated with lower ratings of aggression at age seven, but only for nonadopted children. 22 This link between early environment and child outcome was not found for adopted children, suggesting that passive gene-environment correlations may exist in nonadoptive families that have increased similarity compared to adoptive families. 23

Like other areas of human behavior, parenting itself is subject to genetic influence. This means that “bad parenting” may itself be influenced by the parents’ genetic inheritance. Negative affect, over-control, and even abuse and neglect could be related to the genetic makeup of the parents. This does not mean that environmental factors are unimportant, nor does it make such behavior excusable. It just means that genes can explain parenting behavior to some degree.

It is almost certain that parenting has an environmental influence on children. The fact that parental behavior—including parenting style—may be influenced by genes does not imply that such behaviors have no environmental impact on the children that receive such parenting. What are the best methods for testing the true environmental mediation of the relationship between parent and child behavior? Behavioral genetic designs—adoption and extended twin studies—actually provide the ideal methods for identifying environmental effects while controlling for genetic factors. 24 Behavioral genetic studies have helped resolve the issue of genetic and environmental effects in abusive parenting and its relationship to later behavior problems in children by studying, for example, differences in the physical maltreatment of co-twins. Twin resemblance for maltreatment was substantial and equal for MZ pairs and DZ pairs, suggesting that children’s genetic differences did not elicit abusive parenting. This does not rule out the possibility, however, that parents’ genes may have influenced their abusive parenting. Most importantly, associations between abusive parenting and a child’s later antisocial behavior remained significant even after controlling for genetic differences in the children. 25 It is noteworthy that this genetically informative study provided convincing evidence of an environmental effect of abusive parenting on child outcomes.

The environmental effects of abuse on child development have also been shown to be exacerbated by a child’s genetic predispositions. Children who inherited a deleterious gene that causes a deficiency in monoamine oxidase (MAO-A) appear particularly vulnerable to physical maltreatment, compared to children with a normal MAO-A gene. 26 These findings underscore the importance of genotype X environment interactions, 27 in which genetic predispositions amplify environmental vulnerabilities and vice versa. We can expect that a more detailed understanding of this complex interplay between specific genetic mechanisms and measured environments will emerge over the next few years, as more studies begin to obtain DNA markers of genetic variations.

IV. Other Family Relationships

Behavioral geneticists have also studied family relationships beyond that of the parent and child. Sibling interactions, for example, have been examined in both twin and non-twin siblings. Unlike parents and children, who always share exactly half of their genes, siblings vary in their degree of genetic relatedness. MZ twins are genetically identical; DZ twins and non-twin siblings share about half of their genes, although some pairs may share more or less genetic material. This variation in genetic relatedness could explain why some siblings have a more cooperative and close relationship than others. Genetic similarity among siblings has been shown to affect both their positive and negative interactions with one another, 28 as well as levels of mutual competition and cooperation. 29 In general, siblings who share a stronger genetic makeup demonstrate a closer, more cooperative and positive relationship with one another.

Genetic variations among siblings living in the same family have also been suggested as an important source of differential parenting. The differential parenting of two siblings, albeit stemming originally from their genetic differences, has an environmental effect on the children’s psychological outcomes and may amplify sibling differences over time.

The quality of the relationship between marital partners has also been a subject of behavioral genetic studies. Twin similarity for marital satisfaction has been reported to be greater for MZ pairs than for DZ pairs, 30 suggesting the importance of individual genetic factors in determining the success of a marriage. Indeed, twin studies have also shown significantly greater concordance for divorce among MZ pairs than among DZ pairs, suggesting a substantial genetic effect on the likelihood of a failed marriage. 31 Genetically influenced personality traits, such as negative emotionality (i.e., neuroticism), are also predictive of divorce, and may explain much of the genetic risk for divorce. 32

V. Conclusion

One lesson to be learned from behavioral genetic studies of parenting and other types of family relationships is that one must be careful in drawing conclusions based on findings of family resemblance in nuclear, nonadoptive families. Consider the well-known finding that children of abusers are likely to become aggressive and violent, and perhaps even become abusive parents themselves later in life. 33 Although it is tempting to assume such resemblance is a function of learning and experience, it is possible that inherited genetic factors could explain the transmission of abuse across generations. Family resemblance for a given characteristic does not necessarily imply either genetic or environmental influence, since either could explain observed similarity among family members. Thus, the mere fact that children who are abused by their parents are more likely to become abusive themselves does not prove a causal relationship between parenting behaviors and child outcome. Through genetically controlled studies, we have come to understand that both genes and environment play a role in the cycle of violence. 34 Genes may predispose certain adults toward violence and aggression, even toward their own children. Such behaviors can in turn have a real environmental impact on the child’s mental health and on behavioral outcomes. Children’s genes may also predispose them toward oppositional and other antisocial behaviors, which may elicit negative parenting from the adults who are raising them.

The fact that genetic influences are crucially important for most areas of behavior does not mean that environmental influences are unimportant. Genes typically account for no more than one-half to two-thirds of the variation seen in most individual’s psychological traits. But most environmental influences are based on individual experiences and exposures that are not shared by family members. The implication for families is that most observed resemblance among its individual members is a function of their genetic similarity—not their shared experiences.

Finally, behavioral genetic studies of family relationships provide the valuable information required to develop effective programs of intervention and prevention of serious mental health and behavioral problems. Establishing that environmental effects unequivocally mediate links between parents’ and children’s behavior is a step toward ensuring the success of treatment programs that target either parents or children.

U.S. flag

Official websites use .gov

A .gov website belongs to an official government organization in the United States.

Secure .gov websites use HTTPS

A lock ( ) or https:// means you've safely connected to the .gov website. Share sensitive information only on official, secure websites.

Data and Statistics on Children’s Mental Health

Mental health is an important part of children’s overall health and well-being. Mental health includes children’s mental, emotional, and behavioral well-being. It affects how children think, feel, and act. It also plays a role in how children handle stress, relate to others, and make healthy choices.

Mental disorders among children are described as serious changes in the way children typically learn, behave, or handle their emotions, causing distress and problems getting through the day. 1 Among the more common mental disorders that can be diagnosed in childhood are attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), anxiety, and behavior disorders.

There are different ways to assess mental health and mental disorders in children. CDC uses surveys, like the National Survey of Children’s Health, to describe the presence of positive indicators of children’s mental health and to understand the number of children with diagnosed mental disorders and whether they received treatment. In this type of survey, parents report on indicators of positive mental health for their child and report any diagnoses their child has received from a healthcare provider.  The information on this page provides data about indicators of positive mental health in children and mental health disorders that are most common in children.

Facts about mental health in U.S. children

National data on positive mental health indicators that describe mental, emotional, and behavioral well-being for children are limited. Based on the data we do have:

  • Affection (97.0%), resilience (87.9%), positivity (98.7%) and curiosity (93.9%) among children ages 3-5 years 2
  • Curiosity (93.0%), persistence (84.2%), and self-control (73.8%) among children ages 6-11 years 2
  • Curiosity (86.5 %), persistence (84.7%), and self-control (79.8%) among children ages 12-17 years 2

Facts about mental disorders in U.S. children

  • ADHD 9.8% (approximately 6.0 million) 2
  • Anxiety 9.4% (approximately 5.8 million) 2
  • Behavior problems 8.9% (approximately 5.5 million) 2
  • Depression 4.4% (approximately 2.7 million) 2
  • Having another mental disorder was most common in children with depression: about 3 in 4 children with depression also had anxiety (73.8%) and almost 1 in 2 had behavior problems (47.2%). 3
  • For children with anxiety, more than 1 in 3 also had behavior problems (37.9%) and about 1 in 3 also had depression (32.3%). 3
  • For children with behavior problems, more than 1 in 3 also had anxiety (36.6%) and about 1 in 5 also had depression (20.3%). 3
  • “Ever having been diagnosed with either anxiety or depression” among children aged 6–17 years increased from 5.4% in 2003 to 8% in 2007 and to 8.4% in 2011–2012. 4
  • “Ever having been diagnosed with anxiety” increased from 5.5% in 2007 to 6.4% in 2011–2012. 4
  • “Ever having been diagnosed with depression” did not change between 2007 (4.7%) and 2011-2012 (4.9%). 4
  • For adolescents, depression, substance use and suicide are important concerns. Among adolescents aged 12-17 years in 2018-2019 reporting on the past year:
  • 15.1% had a major depressive episode. 2
  • 36.7% had persistent feelings of sadness or hopelessness. 2
  • 4.1% had a substance use disorder. 2
  • 1.6% had an alcohol use disorder. 2
  • 3.2% had an illicit drug use disorder. 2
  • 18.8% seriously considered attempting suicide. 2
  • 15.7% made a suicide plan. 2
  • 8.9% attempted suicide. 2
  • 2.5% made a suicide attempt requiring medical treatment. 2

Learn more about high-risk substance use among youth . Learn more about suicide .

1 in 6 children aged 2-8 years has a mental, behavioral, or developmental disorder

  • Nearly 8 in 10 children (78.1%) with depression received treatment. 3
  • 6 in 10 children (59.3%) with anxiety received treatment. 3
  • More than 5 in 10 children (53.5%) with behavior disorders received treatment. 3
  • 1 in 6 U.S. children aged 2–8 years (17.4%) had a diagnosed mental, behavioral, or developmental disorder. 5
  • Diagnoses of ADHD, anxiety, and depression become are more common with increased age. 3
  • Behavior problems are more common among children aged 6–11 years than younger or older children. 3

Bar Chart: Mental disorders by age in years - Depression: 3-5 years: 0.1%26#37;, 6-11 years: 1.7%26#37;, 12-17 years: 6.1%26#37; Anxiety: 3-5 years: 1.3%26#37;, 6-11 years: 6.6%26#37;, 12-17 years: 10.5%26#37; Depression: 3-5 years: 3.4%26#37;, 6-11 years: 9.1%26#37;, 12-17 years: 7.5%26#37;

  • Among children aged 2-8 years, boys were more likely than girls to have a mental, behavioral, or developmental disorder. 5
  • Among children living below 100% of the federal poverty level, more than 1 in 5 (22%) had a mental, behavioral, or developmental disorder. 5
  • Age and poverty level affected the likelihood of children receiving treatment for anxiety, depression, or behavior problems. 3
  • Children who were discriminated against based on race or ethnicity had higher percentages of one or more physical health conditions (37.8% versus 27.1%), and one or more mental health conditions (28.9% versus 17.8%). 6
  • Racial/ethnic discrimination was almost seven times as common among children with three other ACEs compared to those with no other ACEs. 6

Note : The estimates reported on this page are based on parent report, using nationally representative surveys. This method has several limitations. It is not known to what extent children receive these diagnoses accurately. Estimates based on parent-reported diagnoses may match those based on medical records, 7  but some children may also have mental disorders that have not been diagnosed, or receive diagnoses that may not be the best fit for their symptoms. Limited information on measuring children’s mental health nationally is available 2 .

Read more about children’s mental health from a community study .

Access to mental health treatment

Early diagnosis and appropriate services for children and their families can make a difference in the lives of children with mental disorders. 7 Access to providers who can offer services, including screening, referrals, and treatment, varies by location. CDC is working to learn more about access to behavioral health services and supports for children and their families.

View information by state describing the rates of different types of providers who can offer behavioral health services providers by county.

View State Specific Provider Data - Map of the United States

Read a recent report describing shortages of services, barriers to treatment, and how integration of behavioral health care with pediatric primary care could address the issues.

Read a policy brief on potential ways to increase access to mental health services for children in rural areas

What is It and Why is It Important?

Data sources for mental health and related conditions

There are many different datasets which include information on children’s mental health and related conditions for children living in the United States.

Healthy People 2030 Healthy People 2030 sets data-driven national objectives to improve health and well-being over the next decade, including children’s mental health and well-being.

National Survey of Family Growth (NSFG) NSFG gathers information on family life, marriage and divorce, pregnancy, infertility, use of contraception, and general and reproductive health.

National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) NHANES assesses health and nutritional status through interviews and physical examinations, and includes conditions, symptoms, and concerns associated with mental health and substance abuse, as well as the use and need for mental health services.

National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) NHIS collects data on children’s mental health, mental disorders, such as ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, depression and anxiety problems, and use and need for mental health services.

National Survey of Children’s Health (NSCH) NSCH examines the health of children, with emphasis on well-being, including medical homes, family interactions, the health of parents, school and after-school experiences, and safe neighborhoods. This survey was redesigned in 2016.

For previous versions of this survey, see also: National Survey of Children’s Health (NSCH 2003, 2007, 2011-12) National Survey of Children with Special Healthcare Needs (NS-CSHCN 2001, 2005-6, 2009-10)

National Survey of the Diagnosis and Treatment of ADHD and Tourette Syndrome (NS-DATA) NS-DATA collects information about children, 2-15 years old in 2011-2012, who had ever been diagnosed with ADHD and/or Tourette syndrome (TS), with the goal of better understanding diagnostic practices, level of impairment, and treatments for this group of children.

National Survey on Drug Use and Health (NSDUH) NSDUH, administered by the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA), provides national- and state-level data on the use of tobacco, alcohol, and illicit drugs (including non-medical use of prescription drugs), as well as data on mental health in the United States.

National Vital Statistics System (NVSS) NVSS contains vital statistics from the official records of live births, deaths, causes of death, marriages, divorces, and annulment recorded by states and independent registration areas

National Youth Tobacco Survey (NYTS) NYTS is a nationally representative school-based survey on tobacco use by public school students enrolled in grades 6-12.

School Associated Violent Death Study (SAVD) SAVD plays an important role in monitoring trends related to school-associated violent deaths (including suicide), identifying the factors that increase the risk, and assessing the effects of prevention efforts.

School Health Policies and Programs Study (SHPPS) SHPPS is a national survey assessing school health policies and practices at the state, district, school, and classroom levels. Collected data includes mental health and social service policies.

Web-based Injury Statistics Query and Reporting System (WISQARS) WISQARS is an interactive database system that provides customized reports of injury-related data.

Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS) The YRBSS monitors health-risk behaviors, including tobacco use, substance abuse, unintentional injuries and violence, sexual behaviors that contribute to unintended pregnancy, and sexually transmitted diseases.

  • Perou R, Bitsko RH, Blumberg SJ, Pastor P, Ghandour RM, Gfroerer JC, Hedden SL, Crosby AE, Visser SN, Schieve LA, Parks SE, Hall JE, Brody D, Simile CM, Thompson WW, Baio J, Avenevoli S, Kogan MD, Huang LN. Mental health surveillance among children – United States, 2005—2011. MMWR 2013;62(Suppl; May 16, 2013):1-35. [ Read summary ]
  • Bitsko RH, Claussen AH, Lichtstein J, Black LJ, Everett Jones S, Danielson MD, Hoenig JM, Davis Jack SP, Brody DJ, Gyawali S, Maenner MM, Warner M, Holland KM, Perou R, Crosby AE, Blumberg SJ, Avenevoli S, Kaminski JW, Ghandour RM. Surveillance of Children’s Mental Health – United States, 2013 – 2019 MMWR, , 2022 / 71(Suppl-2);1–42. [Read article]
  • Ghandour RM, Sherman LJ, Vladutiu CJ, Ali MM, Lynch SE, Bitsko RH, Blumberg SJ. Prevalence and treatment of depression, anxiety, and conduct problems in U.S. children. The Journal of Pediatrics , 2018. Published online before print  October 12, 2018 [ Read summary ]
  • Bitsko RH, Holbrook JR, Ghandour RM, Blumberg SJ, Visser SN, Perou R, Walkup J. Epidemiology and impact of healthcare provider diagnosed anxiety and depression among US children. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics . Published online before print April 24, 2018 [ Read summary ]
  • Cree RA, Bitsko RH, Robinson LR, Holbrook JR, Danielson ML, Smith DS, Kaminski JW, Kenney MK, Peacock G. Health care, family, and community factors associated with mental, behavioral, and developmental disorders and poverty among children aged 2–8 years — United States, 2016. MMWR , 2018;67(5):1377-1383. [ Read article ]
  • Hutchins HJ, Barry CM, Valentine V, Bacon S, Njai R, Claussen AH, Ghandour RM, Lebrun-Harris LA, Perkins K, Robinson LR (submitted). Perceived racial/ethnic discrimination, physical and mental health conditions in childhood, and the relative role of other adverse experiences. Adversity and Resilience Science published online May 23, 2022. [ Read summary ]
  • US Department of Health and Human Services Health Resources and Services Administration & Maternal and Child Health Bureau. Mental health: A report of the Surgeon General . Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, Center for Mental Health Services, and National Institutes of Health, National Institute of Mental Health; 1999. [ Read report ]

To receive email updates about this topic, enter your email address:

Exit Notification / Disclaimer Policy

  • The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) cannot attest to the accuracy of a non-federal website.
  • Linking to a non-federal website does not constitute an endorsement by CDC or any of its employees of the sponsors or the information and products presented on the website.
  • You will be subject to the destination website's privacy policy when you follow the link.
  • CDC is not responsible for Section 508 compliance (accessibility) on other federal or private website.
  • Patient Care & Health Information
  • Diseases & Conditions
  • Depression (major depressive disorder)
  • What is depression? A Mayo Clinic expert explains.

Learn more about depression from Craig Sawchuk, Ph.D., L.P., clinical psychologist at Mayo Clinic.

Hi, I'm Dr. Craig Sawchuk, a clinical psychologist at Mayo Clinic. And I'm here to talk with you about depression. Whether you're looking for answers for yourself, a friend, or loved one, understanding the basics of depression can help you take the next step.

Depression is a mood disorder that causes feelings of sadness that won't go away. Unfortunately, there's a lot of stigma around depression. Depression isn't a weakness or a character flaw. It's not about being in a bad mood, and people who experience depression can't just snap out of it. Depression is a common, serious, and treatable condition. If you're experiencing depression, you're not alone. It honestly affects people of all ages and races and biological sexes, income levels and educational backgrounds. Approximately one in six people will experience a major depressive episode at some point in their lifetime, while up to 16 million adults each year suffer from clinical depression. There are many types of symptoms that make up depression. Emotionally, you may feel sad or down or irritable or even apathetic. Physically, the body really slows down. You feel tired. Your sleep is often disrupted. It's really hard to get yourself motivated. Your thinking also changes. It can just be hard to concentrate. Your thoughts tend to be much more negative. You can be really hard on yourself, feel hopeless and helpless about things. And even in some cases, have thoughts of not wanting to live. Behaviorally, you just want to pull back and withdraw from others, activities, and day-to-day responsibilities. These symptoms all work together to keep you trapped in a cycle of depression. Symptoms of depression are different for everyone. Some symptoms may be a sign of another disorder or medical condition. That's why it's important to get an accurate diagnosis.

While there's no single cause of depression, most experts believe there's a combination of biological, social, and psychological factors that contribute to depression risk. Biologically, we think about genetics or a family history of depression, health conditions such as diabetes, heart disease or thyroid disorders, and even hormonal changes that happen over the lifespan, such as pregnancy and menopause. Changes in brain chemistry, especially disruptions in neurotransmitters like serotonin, that play an important role in regulating many bodily functions, including mood, sleep, and appetite, are thought to play a particularly important role in depression. Socially stressful and traumatic life events, limited access to resources such as food, housing, and health care, and a lack of social support all contribute to depression risk. Psychologically, we think of how negative thoughts and problematic coping behaviors, such as avoidance and substance use, increase our vulnerability to depression.

The good news is that treatment helps. Effective treatments for depression exist and you do have options to see what works best for you. Lifestyle changes that improve sleep habits, exercise, and address underlying health conditions can be an important first step. Medications such as antidepressants can be helpful in alleviating depressive symptoms. Therapy, especially cognitive behavioral therapy, teaches skills to better manage negative thoughts and improve coping behaviors to help break you out of cycles of depression. Whatever the cause, remember that depression is not your fault and it can be treated.

To help diagnose depression, your health care provider may use a physical exam, lab tests, or a mental health evaluation. These results will help identify various treatment options that best fit your situation.

Help is available. You don't have to deal with depression by yourself. Take the next step and reach out. If you're hesitant to talk to a health care provider, talk to a friend or loved one about how to get help. Living with depression isn't easy and you're not alone in your struggles. Always remember that effective treatments and supports are available to help you start feeling better. Want to learn more about depression? Visit mayoclinic.org. Do take care.

Depression is a mood disorder that causes a persistent feeling of sadness and loss of interest. Also called major depressive disorder or clinical depression, it affects how you feel, think and behave and can lead to a variety of emotional and physical problems. You may have trouble doing normal day-to-day activities, and sometimes you may feel as if life isn't worth living.

More than just a bout of the blues, depression isn't a weakness and you can't simply "snap out" of it. Depression may require long-term treatment. But don't get discouraged. Most people with depression feel better with medication, psychotherapy or both.

Depression care at Mayo Clinic

Products & Services

  • A Book: Mayo Clinic Family Health Book, 5th Edition
  • Begin Exploring Women's Health Solutions at Mayo Clinic Store
  • Newsletter: Mayo Clinic Health Letter — Digital Edition

Although depression may occur only once during your life, people typically have multiple episodes. During these episodes, symptoms occur most of the day, nearly every day and may include:

  • Feelings of sadness, tearfulness, emptiness or hopelessness
  • Angry outbursts, irritability or frustration, even over small matters
  • Loss of interest or pleasure in most or all normal activities, such as sex, hobbies or sports
  • Sleep disturbances, including insomnia or sleeping too much
  • Tiredness and lack of energy, so even small tasks take extra effort
  • Reduced appetite and weight loss or increased cravings for food and weight gain
  • Anxiety, agitation or restlessness
  • Slowed thinking, speaking or body movements
  • Feelings of worthlessness or guilt, fixating on past failures or self-blame
  • Trouble thinking, concentrating, making decisions and remembering things
  • Frequent or recurrent thoughts of death, suicidal thoughts, suicide attempts or suicide
  • Unexplained physical problems, such as back pain or headaches

For many people with depression, symptoms usually are severe enough to cause noticeable problems in day-to-day activities, such as work, school, social activities or relationships with others. Some people may feel generally miserable or unhappy without really knowing why.

Depression symptoms in children and teens

Common signs and symptoms of depression in children and teenagers are similar to those of adults, but there can be some differences.

  • In younger children, symptoms of depression may include sadness, irritability, clinginess, worry, aches and pains, refusing to go to school, or being underweight.
  • In teens, symptoms may include sadness, irritability, feeling negative and worthless, anger, poor performance or poor attendance at school, feeling misunderstood and extremely sensitive, using recreational drugs or alcohol, eating or sleeping too much, self-harm, loss of interest in normal activities, and avoidance of social interaction.

Depression symptoms in older adults

Depression is not a normal part of growing older, and it should never be taken lightly. Unfortunately, depression often goes undiagnosed and untreated in older adults, and they may feel reluctant to seek help. Symptoms of depression may be different or less obvious in older adults, such as:

  • Memory difficulties or personality changes
  • Physical aches or pain
  • Fatigue, loss of appetite, sleep problems or loss of interest in sex — not caused by a medical condition or medication
  • Often wanting to stay at home, rather than going out to socialize or doing new things
  • Suicidal thinking or feelings, especially in older men

When to see a doctor

If you feel depressed, make an appointment to see your doctor or mental health professional as soon as you can. If you're reluctant to seek treatment, talk to a friend or loved one, any health care professional, a faith leader, or someone else you trust.

When to get emergency help

If you think you may hurt yourself or attempt suicide, call 911 in the U.S. or your local emergency number immediately.

Also consider these options if you're having suicidal thoughts:

  • Call your doctor or mental health professional.
  • Contact a suicide hotline.
  • In the U.S., call or text 988 to reach the 988 Suicide & Crisis Lifeline, available 24 hours a day, seven days a week. Or use the Lifeline Chat . Services are free and confidential.
  • U.S. veterans or service members who are in crisis can call 988 and then press “1” for the Veterans Crisis Line . Or text 838255. Or chat online .
  • The Suicide & Crisis Lifeline in the U.S. has a Spanish language phone line at 1-888-628-9454 (toll-free).
  • Reach out to a close friend or loved one.
  • Contact a minister, spiritual leader or someone else in your faith community.

If you have a loved one who is in danger of suicide or has made a suicide attempt, make sure someone stays with that person. Call 911 or your local emergency number immediately. Or, if you think you can do so safely, take the person to the nearest hospital emergency room.

More Information

Depression (major depressive disorder) care at Mayo Clinic

  • Male depression: Understanding the issues
  • Nervous breakdown: What does it mean?
  • Pain and depression: Is there a link?

There is a problem with information submitted for this request. Review/update the information highlighted below and resubmit the form.

From Mayo Clinic to your inbox

Sign up for free and stay up to date on research advancements, health tips, current health topics, and expertise on managing health. Click here for an email preview.

Error Email field is required

Error Include a valid email address

To provide you with the most relevant and helpful information, and understand which information is beneficial, we may combine your email and website usage information with other information we have about you. If you are a Mayo Clinic patient, this could include protected health information. If we combine this information with your protected health information, we will treat all of that information as protected health information and will only use or disclose that information as set forth in our notice of privacy practices. You may opt-out of email communications at any time by clicking on the unsubscribe link in the e-mail.

Thank you for subscribing!

You'll soon start receiving the latest Mayo Clinic health information you requested in your inbox.

Sorry something went wrong with your subscription

Please, try again in a couple of minutes

It's not known exactly what causes depression. As with many mental disorders, a variety of factors may be involved, such as:

  • Biological differences. People with depression appear to have physical changes in their brains. The significance of these changes is still uncertain, but may eventually help pinpoint causes.
  • Brain chemistry. Neurotransmitters are naturally occurring brain chemicals that likely play a role in depression. Recent research indicates that changes in the function and effect of these neurotransmitters and how they interact with neurocircuits involved in maintaining mood stability may play a significant role in depression and its treatment.
  • Hormones. Changes in the body's balance of hormones may be involved in causing or triggering depression. Hormone changes can result with pregnancy and during the weeks or months after delivery (postpartum) and from thyroid problems, menopause or a number of other conditions.
  • Inherited traits. Depression is more common in people whose blood relatives also have this condition. Researchers are trying to find genes that may be involved in causing depression.
  • Marijuana and depression
  • Vitamin B-12 and depression

Risk factors

Depression often begins in the teens, 20s or 30s, but it can happen at any age. More women than men are diagnosed with depression, but this may be due in part because women are more likely to seek treatment.

Factors that seem to increase the risk of developing or triggering depression include:

  • Certain personality traits, such as low self-esteem and being too dependent, self-critical or pessimistic
  • Traumatic or stressful events, such as physical or sexual abuse, the death or loss of a loved one, a difficult relationship, or financial problems
  • Blood relatives with a history of depression, bipolar disorder, alcoholism or suicide
  • Being lesbian, gay, bisexual or transgender, or having variations in the development of genital organs that aren't clearly male or female (intersex) in an unsupportive situation
  • History of other mental health disorders, such as anxiety disorder, eating disorders or post-traumatic stress disorder
  • Abuse of alcohol or recreational drugs
  • Serious or chronic illness, including cancer, stroke, chronic pain or heart disease
  • Certain medications, such as some high blood pressure medications or sleeping pills (talk to your doctor before stopping any medication)

Complications

Depression is a serious disorder that can take a terrible toll on you and your family. Depression often gets worse if it isn't treated, resulting in emotional, behavioral and health problems that affect every area of your life.

Examples of complications associated with depression include:

  • Excess weight or obesity, which can lead to heart disease and diabetes
  • Pain or physical illness
  • Alcohol or drug misuse
  • Anxiety, panic disorder or social phobia
  • Family conflicts, relationship difficulties, and work or school problems
  • Social isolation
  • Suicidal feelings, suicide attempts or suicide
  • Self-mutilation, such as cutting
  • Premature death from medical conditions
  • Depression and anxiety: Can I have both?

There's no sure way to prevent depression. However, these strategies may help.

  • Take steps to control stress, to increase your resilience and boost your self-esteem.
  • Reach out to family and friends, especially in times of crisis, to help you weather rough spells.
  • Get treatment at the earliest sign of a problem to help prevent depression from worsening.
  • Consider getting long-term maintenance treatment to help prevent a relapse of symptoms.
  • Brown AY. Allscripts EPSi. Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. Nov. 17, 2016.
  • Research report: Psychiatry and psychology, 2016-2017. Mayo Clinic. http://www.mayo.edu/research/departments-divisions/department-psychiatry-psychology/overview?_ga=1.199925222.939187614.1464371889. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Depressive disorders. In: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington, Va.: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. http://www.psychiatryonline.org. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Depression. National Institute of Mental Health. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/topics/depression/index.shtml. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Depression. National Alliance on Mental Illness. http://www.nami.org/Learn-More/Mental-Health-Conditions/Depression/Overview. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Depression: What you need to know. National Institute of Mental Health. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/publications/depression-what-you-need-to-know/index.shtml. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • What is depression? American Psychiatric Association. https://www.psychiatry.org/patients-families/depression/what-is-depression. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Depression. NIH Senior Health. https://nihseniorhealth.gov/depression/aboutdepression/01.html. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Children’s mental health: Anxiety and depression. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/childrensmentalhealth/depression.html#depression. Accessed. Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Depression and complementary health approaches: What the science says. National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health. https://nccih.nih.gov/health/providers/digest/depression-science. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Depression. Natural Medicines. https://naturalmedicines.therapeuticresearch.com/databases/medical-conditions/d/depression.aspx. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Natural medicines in the clinical management of depression. Natural Medicines. http://naturaldatabase.therapeuticresearch.com/ce/CECourse.aspx?cs=naturalstandard&s=ND&pm=5&pc=15-111. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • The road to resilience. American Psychological Association. http://www.apa.org/helpcenter/road-resilience.aspx. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Simon G, et al. Unipolar depression in adults: Choosing initial treatment. http://www.uptodate.com/home. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Stewart D, et al. Risks of antidepressants during pregnancy: Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). http://www.uptodate.com/home. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Kimmel MC, et al. Safety of infant exposure to antidepressants and benzodiazepines through breastfeeding. http://www.uptodate.com/home. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Bipolar and related disorders. In: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington, Va.: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. http://www.psychiatryonline.org. Accessed Jan. 23, 2017.
  • Hirsch M, et al. Monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs) for treating depressed adults. http://www.uptodate.com/home. Accessed Jan. 24, 2017.
  • Hall-Flavin DK (expert opinion). Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. Jan. 31, 2017.
  • Krieger CA (expert opinion). Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minn. Feb. 2, 2017.
  • Antidepressant withdrawal: Is there such a thing?
  • Antidepressants and alcohol: What's the concern?
  • Antidepressants and weight gain: What causes it?
  • Antidepressants: Can they stop working?
  • Antidepressants: Selecting one that's right for you
  • Antidepressants: Side effects
  • Antidepressants: Which cause the fewest sexual side effects?
  • Atypical antidepressants
  • Clinical depression: What does that mean?
  • Depression in women: Understanding the gender gap
  • Depression, anxiety and exercise
  • Depression: Supporting a family member or friend
  • MAOIs and diet: Is it necessary to restrict tyramine?
  • Monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs)
  • Natural remedies for depression: Are they effective?
  • Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs)
  • Serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs)
  • Treatment-resistant depression
  • Tricyclic antidepressants and tetracyclic antidepressants

Associated Procedures

  • Complete blood count (CBC)
  • Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT)
  • Psychotherapy
  • Transcranial magnetic stimulation
  • Vagus nerve stimulation

News from Mayo Clinic

  • Mayo Clinic Q and A: How to support a loved one with depression Oct. 23, 2022, 11:00 a.m. CDT
  • Mayo study lays foundation to predict antidepressant response in people with suicide attempts Oct. 03, 2022, 03:30 p.m. CDT
  • Science Saturday: Researchers validate threshold for determining effectiveness of antidepressant treatment Aug. 27, 2022, 11:00 a.m. CDT

Mayo Clinic in Rochester, Minnesota, has been recognized as one of the top Psychiatry hospitals in the nation for 2023-2024 by U.S. News & World Report.

  • Symptoms & causes
  • Diagnosis & treatment
  • Doctors & departments
  • Care at Mayo Clinic

Mayo Clinic does not endorse companies or products. Advertising revenue supports our not-for-profit mission.

  • Opportunities

Mayo Clinic Press

Check out these best-sellers and special offers on books and newsletters from Mayo Clinic Press .

  • Mayo Clinic on Incontinence - Mayo Clinic Press Mayo Clinic on Incontinence
  • The Essential Diabetes Book - Mayo Clinic Press The Essential Diabetes Book
  • Mayo Clinic on Hearing and Balance - Mayo Clinic Press Mayo Clinic on Hearing and Balance
  • FREE Mayo Clinic Diet Assessment - Mayo Clinic Press FREE Mayo Clinic Diet Assessment
  • Mayo Clinic Health Letter - FREE book - Mayo Clinic Press Mayo Clinic Health Letter - FREE book

We’re transforming healthcare

Make a gift now and help create new and better solutions for more than 1.3 million patients who turn to Mayo Clinic each year.

On the Usefulness of Behavior Genetics: Using Family Studies in Evolutionary Psychological Science to Improve Causal Inference and Sharpen Theory

  • Published: 26 October 2023
  • Volume 9 , pages 387–399, ( 2023 )

Cite this article

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

  • Brian B. Boutwell 1 &
  • Dario Maestripieri 2  

171 Accesses

Explore all metrics

We argue that research in the psychological sciences testing evolutionarily informed questions could benefit considerably from more frequent use of techniques common in behavior genetics.

We review some of the reasons why data and analytical strategies in behavior genetics confer certain advantages over more traditional forms of data analysis. In particular, we focus on the wide availability of secondary data, the generalizability of data, the capacity of certain designs to bolster causal inference capabilities, and the overall adaptability of the research designs to a wide array of empirical questions.

Not only do we show how the use of sibling designs can be of methodological assistance, but we also demonstrate how they can play a role in refining theories informed by evolution. In order to give a more concrete vision of what this can look like, we offer a type of case study using prior work which has already taken advantage of behavior genetic tools.

Conclusions

Because of the efforts to situate psychological science in the context of evolutionary biology, the field has undergone considerable intellectual growth. We suggest that by simply making more frequent use of tools in behavior genetics, the fields of psychology might further accelerate the progress that is already well underway.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Carl Menger, F.A. Hayek and the evolutionary strand in Austrian economics

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Organisms and the Causes and Consequences of Selection: A Reply to Vidya et al.

two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

Behavioural Genetics

Data availability.

Not applicable.

Bailey, J. M. (1997). Are genetically based individual differences compatible with species- wide adaptations? In N. L. Segal, G. E. Weisfeld, & C. C. Weisfeld (Eds.), Uniting psychology and biology: Integrative perspectives on human development (pp. 81–100). American Psychological Association.

Balthazart, J. (2011). The Biology of Homosexuality . Oxford University Press.

Banta, J. A., & Richards, C. L. (2018). Quantitative epigenetics and evolution. Heredity , 121 (3), 210–224.

Article   Google Scholar  

Barbaro, N., Boutwell, B. B., Barnes, J. C., & Shackelford, T. K. (2017). Genetic confounding of the relationship between father absence and age at menarche. Evolution and Human Behavior , 38 (3), 357–365.

Barnes, J. C., & Boutwell, B. B. (2013). A demonstration of the generalizability of twin-based research on antisocial behavior. Behavior Genetics , 43 , 120–131.

Barnes, J. C., Boutwell, B. B., Beaver, K. M., Gibson, C. L., & Wright, J. P. (2014a). On the consequences of ignoring genetic influences in criminological research. Journal of Criminal Justice , 42 (6), 471–482.

Barnes, J. C., Wright, J. P., Boutwell, B. B., Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, E. J., Nedelec, J. L., & Beaver, K. M. (2014b). Demonstrating the validity of twin research in criminology. Criminology , 52 (4), 588–626.

Boutwell, B. B., Narvey, C. S., Helton, J. J., & Piquero, A. R. (2022). Why twin studies are important for health span science research: The case of maltreatment of aging adults. BMC Geriatrics , 22 (1), 943.

Burt, S. A., Donnellan, M. B., Humbad, M. N., Hicks, B. M., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2010). Does marriage inhibit antisocial behavior? An examination of selection vs causation via a longitudinal twin design. Archives of General Psychiatry , 67 (12), 1309–1315.

Buss, D. M. (1984). Evolutionary biology and personality psychology: Toward a conception of human nature and individual differences. American Psychologist , 39 (10), 1135–1147.

Buss, D. M. (1987). Evolutionary hypotheses and behavioral genetic methods: Hopes for a union of two disparate disciplines. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 10 (1), 20–20.

Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 12 (1), 1–14.

Buss, D. M. (2018). Sexual and emotional infidelity: Evolved gender differences in jealousy prove robust and replicable. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 13 (2), 155–160.

Christensen, K., Vaupel, J. W., Holm, N. V., & Yashin, A. I. (1995). Mortality among twins after age 6: Fetal origins hypothesis versus twin method. Bmj , 310 (6977), 432–436.

Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life . Murray.

Dennett, D. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea: Evolution and the meanings of Life . Penguin.

Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin , 130 , 920–958.

Falconer, D. S., & Mackay, T. F. C. (1996). Introduction to quantitative Genetics (4th ed.). Longan Group Ltd.

Freedman, D. G. (1968). An evolutionary framework for behavioral research. In S. G. Vandenberg (Ed.), Progress in human behavior genetics (1–6pp. 1–6 vol.). John Hopkins University Press.

Freedman, D. G. (1971). An evolutionary approach to research on the life cycle. Human Development , 14 (2), 87–99.

Ganna, A., & Zietsch, B. P. (2019). Large-scale GWAS reveals insights into the genetic architecture of same-sex sexual behavior. Science , 365 (6456), eaat7693. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aat7693 .

Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain , 123 (7), 1293–1326.

Gonzalez, J. E., & Ferrer, E. (2015). Efficacy of methods for ovulation estimation and their effect on the statistical detection of ovulation-linked behavioral fluctuations. Behavior Research Methods , 48 , 1125–1144.

Hemani, G., Knott, S., & Haley, C. (2013). An evolutionary perspective on epistasis and the missing heritability. PLoS Genetics , 9(2), e1003295.

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 33 (2–3), 1–75.

Google Scholar  

Hill, W. G., Goddard, M. E., & Visscher, P. M. (2008). Data and theory point to mainly additive genetic variance for complex traits. PLoS Genetics , 4(2), e1000008.

Jünger, J., Motta-Mena, N. V., Cardenas, R., Bailey, D., Rosenfield, K. A., Schild, C., & Puts, D. A. (2018). Do women’s preferences for masculine voices shift across the ovulatory cycle? Hormones and Behavior , 106 , 122–134.

Kavish, N., Connolly, E. J., & Boutwell, B. B. (2019). Genetic and environmental contributions to the association between violent victimization and major depressive disorder. Personality and Individual Differences , 140 , 103–110.

Knopik, V. S., Neiderhiser, J. M., DeFries, J. C., & Plomin, R. (2016). Behavioral genetics (7th ed.). Macmillan Higher Education.

Lee, J. J. (2012). Correlation and causation in the study of personality. European Journal of Personality , 26 , 372–412.

Lukaszewski, A. W., Lewis, D. M., Durkee, P. K., Sell, A. N., Sznycer, D., & Buss, D. M. (2020). An adaptationist framework for personality science. European Journal of Personality , 34 (6), 1151–1174.

Mackay, T. F. (2014). Epistasis and quantitative traits: Using model organisms to study gene–gene interactions. Nature Reviews Genetics , 15 (1), 22–33.

Maestripieri, D., & Boutwell, B. B. (2022). Human nature and personality variation: Reconnecting evolutionary psychology with the science of individual differences. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews , 143 , 104946.

McAdams, T. A., Rijsdijk, F. V., Zavos, H. M., & Pingault, J. B. (2021). Twins and causal inference: Leveraging nature’s experiment. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine , 11 , a039552.

McGue, M., Osler, M., & Christensen, K. (2010). Causal inference and observational research: The utility of twins. Perspectives on Psychological Science , 5 (5), 546–556.

Međedovic, J. (2023). Why human behavioral ecology needs behavioral genetics: The problem of phenotypic gambit. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences , in press.

Mendle, J., Turkheimer, E., D’Onofrio, B. M., Lynch, S. K., Emery, R. E., Slutske, W. S., & Martin, N. G. (2006). Family structure and age at menarche: A children-of-twins approach. Developmental Psychology , 42 (3), 533–542.

Nesse, R. M. (1984). An evolutionary perspective on psychiatry. Comprehensive Psychiatry , 25 (6), 575–580.

Nesse, R. M. (2009). Evolution at 150: Time for truly biological psychiatry. The British Journal of Psychiatry , 195 (6), 471–472.

Novak, A., Boutwell, B. B., & Smith, T. B. (2023). Taking the problem of colliders seriously in the study of crime: A research note. Journal of Experimental Criminology . https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-023-09565-x .

Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science , 349 , https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716 .

Pearl, J. (2009). Causal inference in statistics: An overview. Statistics Surveys , 3 , 96–146. https://doi.org/10.1214/09-SS057 .

Penke, L. (2010). Bridging the gap between modern evolutionary psychology and the study of individual differences. In D. M. Buss, & P. Hawley (Eds.), The evolution of personality and individual differences (pp. 243–279). Oxford University Press.

Penke, L., & Jokela, M. (2016). The evolutionary genetics of personality revisited. Current Opinion in Psychology , 7 , 104–109.

Penke, L., Denissen, J. J. A., & Miller, G. F. (2007). The evolutionary genetics of personality. European Journal of Personality , 21 , 549–587.

Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct . William Morrow & Co.

Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works . Viking.

Pinker, S. (2002). The blank Slate: The modern denial of human nature . Viking.

Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences , 10 (1), 1–60.

Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M., & Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics , 47 (7), 702–709.

Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior , 26 (5), 388–397.

Richardson, G. B., McGee, N., & Copping, L. T. (2021). Advancing the psychometric study of human life history indicators. Human Nature , 32 , 363–386.

Rohrer, J. M. (2018). Thinking clearly about correlations and causation: Graphical causal models for observational data. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science , 1 (1), 27–42.

Sanz-de-Galdeano, A., & Terskaya, A. (2023). Sibling differences in genetic propensity for education: How do parents react? The Review of Economics and Statistics , 1–44. https://doi.org/10.1162/rest_a_01326 .

Segal, N. L. (1993). Twin, sibling, and adoption methods: Tests of evolutionary hypotheses. American Psychologist , 48 (9), 943–956.

Shrout, P. E., & Rodgers, J. L. (2018). Psychology, science, and knowledge construction: Broadening perspectives from the replication crisis. Annual Review of Psychology , 69 , 487–510.

Sjölander, A., Frisell, T., & Öberg, S. (2012). Causal interpretation of between-within models for twin research. Epidemiologic Methods , 1 (1), 217–237.

Smaldino, P. E., Lukaszewski, A., von Rueden, C., & Gurven, M. (2019). Niche diversity can explain cross-cultural differences in personality structure. Nature Human Behaviour , 3 (12), 1276–1283.

Taji, W., Scrivner, C., & Maestripieri, D. (2020). Toward a general theory of human individual differences: Can evolutionary psychology meet the challenge? Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences , 14 , 384–389.

Tanksley, P. T., Barnes, J. C., Boutwell, B. B., Arseneault, L., Caspi, A., Danese, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2020). Identifying psychological pathways to polyvictimization: Evidence from a longitudinal cohort study of twins from the UK. Journal of Experimental Criminology , 16 , 431–461.

Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). On the universality of human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: The role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality , 58 , 17–67.

Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 9 (5), 160–164.

VanderWeele, T. J. (2019). Principles of confounder selection. European Journal of Epidemiology , 34 , 211–219.

VanderWeele, T. J. (2021). Can sophisticated study designs with regression analyses of observational data provide causal inferences? JAMA Psychiatry , 78 (3), 244–246.

Verweij, K. J., & Zietsch, B. P. (2012). Maintenance of genetic variation in human personality: Testing evolutionary models by estimating heritability due to common causal variants and investigating the effect of distant inbreeding. Evolution , 66 (10), 3238–3251.

Walter, K. V., Conroy-Beam, D., Buss, D. M., Asao, K., Sorokowska, A., Sorokowski, P., & Zupančič, M. (2020). Sex differences in mate preferences across 45 countries: A large-scale replication. Psychological Science , 31 (4), 408–423.

Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleiotropy, natural selection, and the evolution of senescence. Evolution , 11 , 398–411.

Willoughby, E. A., Polderman, T. C., & Boutwell, B. B. (2023). Behavioural genetics methods. Nature Reviews Methods Primers , 3 , 10.

Zietsch, B. P., & Sidari, M. J. (2020). A critique of life history approaches to human trait covariation. Evolution and Human Behavior , 41 (6), 527–535.

Zietsch, B. P., Morley, K. I., Shekar, S. N., Verweij, K. J., Keller, M. C., Macgregor, S., & Martin, N. G. (2008). Genetic factors predisposing to homosexuality may increase mating success in heterosexuals. Evolution and Human Behavior , 29 (6), 424–433.

Zietsch, B. P., de Candia, T. R., & Keller, M. C. (2015). Evolutionary behavioral genetics. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences , 2 , 73–80.

Zietsch, B. P., Sidari, M. J., Murphy, S. C., Sherlock, J. M., & Lee, A. J. (2021a). For the good of evolutionary psychology, let’s reunite proximate and ultimate explanations. Evolution and Human Behavior , 42 (1), 76–78.

Zietsch, B. P., Sidari, M. J., Abdellaoui, A., Maier, R., Långström, N., Guo, S., & Verweij, K. J. (2021b). Genomic evidence consistent with antagonistic pleiotropy may help explain the evolutionary maintenance of same-sex sexual behaviour in humans. Nature Human Behaviour , 5 (9), 1251–1258.

Download references

The authors have no funding to report.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

The University of Mississippi, University of Mississippi Medical Center, University, MS, USA

Brian B. Boutwell

The University of Chicago, Chicago, USA

Dario Maestripieri

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

BB and DM contributed equally to both the conception of the idea and the process of writing the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Brian B. Boutwell .

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical Approval

Competing interests, additional information, publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Boutwell, B.B., Maestripieri, D. On the Usefulness of Behavior Genetics: Using Family Studies in Evolutionary Psychological Science to Improve Causal Inference and Sharpen Theory. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 9 , 387–399 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-023-00228-9

Download citation

Received : 03 August 2023

Revised : 06 October 2023

Accepted : 10 October 2023

Published : 26 October 2023

Issue Date : December 2023

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-023-00228-9

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Behavior Genetics
  • Research Design
  • Psychological Science
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

IMAGES

  1. PPT

    two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

  2. PPT

    two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

  3. Behavior Genetics Dr. Blakemore PSY ppt download

    two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

  4. PPT

    two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

  5. Nature and Nurture What is the difference between behavior genetics

    two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

  6. PPT

    two types of research studies often used by behavior geneticists

VIDEO

  1. Sample Design

  2. Genetics explained under 60 seconds

  3. Types of research

  4. Research Design

  5. What Genetic Thread Do These Six Strangers Have in Common?

  6. Is Your pet's Personality Influenced by Coat Color?

COMMENTS

  1. Behavioral Genetics

    Behavioral genetics is the study of the influence one's genetic makeup and lived experiences within their unique environment have on their current behavior. Many use the term nature versus nurture ...

  2. Overview of Behavioral Genetics Research for Family Researchers

    A twin study is most often used. In the twin design, researchers compare results across monozygotic twins (who share 100% of their DNA) and dizygotic twins (who share 50% of their DNA, on average; e.g., Iacono & McGue, 2002). Another type of behavioral genetics study design is an adoption study (e.g., McGue et al., 2007). In this type of study ...

  3. Behavioral Genetics

    Behavioral geneticists study how individual differences arise, in the present, through the interaction of genes and the environment. When studying human behavior, behavioral geneticists often employ twin and adoption studies to research questions of interest. Twin studies compare the rates that a given behavioral trait is shared among identical ...

  4. Behavior genetics

    The first human behavioral genetic research on intelligence and mental illness began in the 1920s, when environmentalism (the theory that behaviour is a result of nongenetic factors such as various childhood experiences) became popular and before Nazi Germany's abuse of genetics made the notion of hereditary influence abhorrent.Although genetic research on human behaviour continued ...

  5. Celebrating a Century of Research in Behavioral Genetics

    The most important development during this century of behavioral genetic research has been the synthesis of the two worlds of genetics, quantitative genetics and molecular genetics. Quantitative genetics and molecular genetics both have their origins in the 1860s with Francis Galton (Galton 1865 , 1869 ) and Gregor Mendel (Mendel 1866 ...

  6. Behavioural genetics

    Behavioural genetics, also referred to as behaviour genetics, is a field of scientific research that uses genetic methods to investigate the nature and origins of individual differences in behaviour.While the name "behavioural genetics" connotes a focus on genetic influences, the field broadly investigates the extent to which genetic and environmental factors influence individual differences ...

  7. 4.3 Behavioral Genetics

    Learning Outcomes. Behavioral geneticists study how individual differences arise, in the present, through the interaction of genes and the environment. When studying human behavior, behavioral geneticists often employ twin and adoption studies to research questions of interest. Twin studies compare the rates that a given behavioral trait is ...

  8. PDF Behavior Genetic Research Methods

    Behavioral genetics, of course, is simply the science of genetics in its many forms as it is applied to behavior, and for the most part there is no reason for genetic research methods for the study of behavior to be any different than genetic methods applied to nonbehavioral characteristics of organisms. The genetics of behavior can be studied in

  9. Behavioral Genomics

    Such questions are fundamental to the study of behavioral genomics. The first systematic studies of whether human behaviors are hereditary were conducted over 100 years ago by Sir Francis Galton ...

  10. Behavioural genetics methods

    Abstract. The question of why people show individual differences in their behaviours and capacities has intrigued researchers for centuries. Behaviour genetics offers us various methods to address ...

  11. Behavioral Genetics

    The field of behavioral genetics, or more specifically human behavioral genetics, is concerned with the study of the causes of individual differences in psychological traits, such as intelligence (IQ) and personality. It also looks into to the possible role of genetic factors as a cause of abnormal behavior and psychiatric disorders such as ...

  12. Genes, behavior, and behavior genetics

    The validity of behavior genetics as a discipline depends upon the validity of the research methods used to justify such claims. It also depends, foundationally, upon certain key assumptions concerning the relationship between genotype (one's specific DNA sequences) and phenotype (any and all observable traits or characteristics).

  13. Chapter eight

    Chapter eight - Behavior Genetic Research Methods. ... Research methods in both behavioral genetics and personality are currently at a crossroads. This chapter examines the disagreement about the genetics of behavior by reformulating its methodological foundation of twin and family studies. It applies the reformulation of older methods to gain ...

  14. Behavioral Genetics

    For example, the Canine Behavioral Assessment & Research Questionnaire (C-BARQ) is frequently used for the behavior phenotyping in genetic studies of dogs (Zapata et al. 2016). As advantages, questionnaires are a useful tool to acquire large sample sizes and assess every-day behaviors that are not biased by a test setting.

  15. Top 10 Replicated Findings from Behavioral Genetics

    Finally, we note that four of the top-10 findings (2, 7, 8 and 9) are about environmental influences rather than genetic influences. By using genetically sensitive designs such as twin studies, behavioral genetics has revealed almost as much about the environment as about genetics. 1. All psychological traits show significant and substantial ...

  16. Using genetics for social science

    Abstract. Social science genetics is concerned with understanding whether, how and why genetic differences between human beings are linked to differences in behaviours and socioeconomic outcomes ...

  17. PDF Celebrating a Century of Research in Behavioral Genetics

    insights led to methods to study genetic influence on com-plex traits and when Mendel's work was re-discovered. The ... genetic and molecular genetic research on behavior, a whis - ... Fig. 1 Synthesis of the two worlds of genetics: from behavioral genetics to behavioral genomics. 1 3 76. Behavior Genetics (2023) 53:75-84 ...

  18. Behavioral Genetics--A second look at twin studies

    A second look at twin studies. As behavioral genetics enters a second century, the field's oldest research method remains both relevant and controversial. ... the approximately two-fold difference in genetic similarity between the two types of twins should outweigh any complications, says John Hewitt, PhD, director of the Institute for ...

  19. Twin Studies and Behavior Genetics

    The vast majority of human behaviors and characteristics are complex traits and are influenced by both genetic and environmental influences, as well as the interplay between these two. Based on the differing genetic relatedness of monozygotic co-twins and dizygotic co-twins, the classical twin model allows for an estimation of the relative ...

  20. Behavioral genetic methods

    Traditional behavioral genetic methods involve the use primarily of family, twin, and adoption correlations to estimate the relative contributions of genetic and environmental influences in the etiology of individual differences. These methods and representative results for personality are described. However, newer methods are emphasized ...

  21. The Biology of Relationships: What Behavioral Genetics Tells Us About

    The general strategy in behavioral-genetic research designs involves the study of family members with varying degrees of genetic and environmental relatedness. 3 For example, genetic influences in a trait are evident if pairs of monozygotic (MZ) twins (who are genetically identical) are more similar to one another than dizygotic (DZ) twins (who ...

  22. Data and Statistics on Children's Mental Health

    Many family, community, and healthcare factors are related to children's mental health. Among children aged 2-8 years, boys were more likely than girls to have a mental, behavioral, or developmental disorder. 5 Among children living below 100% of the federal poverty level, more than 1 in 5 (22%) had a mental, behavioral, or developmental disorder. 5 ...

  23. Celebrating a Century of Research in Behavioral Genetics

    A century after the first twin and adoption studies of behavior in the 1920s, this review looks back on the journey and celebrates milestones in behavioral genetic research. After a whistle-stop tour of early quantitative genetic research and the parallel journey of molecular genetics, the travelogue focuses on the last fifty years. Just as quantitative genetic discoveries were beginning to ...

  24. Depression (major depressive disorder)

    Anxiety, agitation or restlessness. Slowed thinking, speaking or body movements. Feelings of worthlessness or guilt, fixating on past failures or self-blame. Trouble thinking, concentrating, making decisions and remembering things. Frequent or recurrent thoughts of death, suicidal thoughts, suicide attempts or suicide.

  25. On the Usefulness of Behavior Genetics: Using Family Studies ...

    Objectives We argue that research in the psychological sciences testing evolutionarily informed questions could benefit considerably from more frequent use of techniques common in behavior genetics. Methods We review some of the reasons why data and analytical strategies in behavior genetics confer certain advantages over more traditional forms of data analysis. In particular, we focus on the ...