- Search Search Please fill out this field.
What Are Heuristics?
Understanding heuristics.
- Pros and Cons
- Examples in Behavioral Economics
Heuristics and Psychology
The bottom line.
- Investing Basics
Heuristics: Definition, Pros & Cons, and Examples
James Chen, CMT is an expert trader, investment adviser, and global market strategist.
Heuristics are mental shortcuts that help people make quick decisions. They are rules or methods that help people use reason and past experience to solve problems efficiently. Commonly used to simplify problems and avoid cognitive overload, heuristics are part of how the human brain evolved and is wired, allowing individuals to quickly reach reasonable conclusions or solutions to complex problems. These solutions may not be optimal ones but are often sufficient given limited timeframes and calculative capacity.
These cognitive shortcuts feature prominently in behavioral economics .
Key Takeaways
- Heuristics are mental shortcuts for solving problems in a quick way that delivers a result that is sufficient enough to be useful given time constraints.
- Investors and financial professionals use a heuristic approach to speed up analysis and investment decisions.
- Heuristics can lead to poor decision-making based on a limited data set, but the speed of decisions can sometimes make up for the disadvantages.
- Behavioral economics has focused on heuristics as one limitation of human beings behaving like rational actors.
- Availability, anchoring, confirmation bias, and the hot hand fallacy are some examples of heuristics people use in their economic lives.
Investopedia / Danie Drankwalter
People employ heuristics naturally due to the evolution of the human brain. The brain can only process so much information at once and therefore must employ various shortcuts or practical rules of thumb . We would not get very far if we had to stop to think about every little detail or collect every piece of available information and integrate it into an analysis.
Heuristics therefore facilitate timely decisions that may not be the absolute best ones but are appropriate enough. Individuals are constantly using this sort of intelligent guesswork, trial and error, process of elimination, and past experience to solve problems or chart a course of action. In a world that is increasingly complex and overloaded with big data, heuristic methods make decision-making simpler and faster through shortcuts and good-enough calculations.
First identified in economics by the political scientist and organizational scholar Herbert Simon in his work on bounded rationality, heuristics have now become a cornerstone of behavioral economics.
Rather than subscribing to the idea that economic behavior was rational and based upon all available information to secure the best possible outcome for an individual ("optimizing"), Simon believed decision-making was about achieving outcomes that were "good enough" for the individual based on their limited information and balancing the interests of others. Simon called this " satisficing ," a portmanteau of the words "satisfy" and "suffice."
Advantages and Disadvantages of Using Heuristics
The main advantage to using heuristics is that they allow people to make good enough decisions without having all of the information and without having to undertake complex calculations.
Because humans cannot possibly obtain or process all the information needed to make fully rational decisions, they instead seek to use the information they do have to produce a satisfactory result, or one that is good enough. Heuristics allow people to go beyond their cognitive limits.
Heuristics are also advantageous when speed or timeliness matters—for example, deciding to enter a trade or making a snap judgment about some important decision. Heuristics are thus handy when there is no time to carefully weigh all options and their merits.
Disadvantages
There are also drawbacks to using heuristics. While they may be quick and dirty, they will likely not produce the optimal decision and can also be wrong entirely. Quick decisions without all the information can lead to errors in judgment, and miscalculations can lead to mistakes.
Moreover, heuristics leave us prone to biases that tend to lead us toward irrational economic behavior and sway our understanding of the world. Such heuristics have been identified and cataloged by the field of behavioral economics.
Quick & easy
Allows decision-making that goes beyond our cognitive capacity
Allows for snap judgments when time is limited
Often inaccurate
Can lead to systemic biases or errors in judgment
Example of Heuristics in Behavioral Economics
Representativeness.
A popular shortcut method in problem-solving identified in behavioral economics is called representativeness heuristics. Representativeness uses mental shortcuts to make decisions based on past events or traits that are representative of or similar to the current situation.
Say, for example, Fast Food ABC expanded its operations to India and its stock price soared. An analyst noted that India is a profitable venture for all fast-food chains. Therefore, when Fast Food XYZ announced its plan to explore the Indian market the following year, the analyst wasted no time in giving XYZ a "buy" recommendation.
Although their shortcut approach saved reviewing data for both companies, it may not have been the best decision. Fast Food XYZ may have food that is not appealing to Indian consumers, which research would have revealed.
Anchoring and Adjustment
Anchoring and adjustment is another prevalent heuristic approach. With anchoring and adjustment, a person begins with a specific target number or value—called the anchor—and subsequently adjusts that number until an acceptable value is reached over time. The major problem with this method is that if the value of the initial anchor is not the true value, then all subsequent adjustments will be systematically biased toward the anchor and away from the true value.
An example of anchoring and adjustment is a car salesman beginning negotiations with a very high price (that is arguably well above the fair value ). Because the high price is an anchor, the final price will tend to be higher than if the car salesman had offered a fair or low price to start.
Availability (Recency) Heuristic
The availability (or recency) heuristic is an issue where people give too much weight to the probability of an event happening again if it recently has occurred. For instance, if a shark attack is reported in the news, those headlines make the event salient and can lead people to stay away from the water, even though shark attacks remain very rare.
Another example is the case of the " hot hand ," or the sense that following a string of successes, an individual is likely to continue being successful. Whether at the casino, in the markets, or playing basketball, the hot hand has been debunked. A string of recent good luck does not alter the overall probability of events occurring.
Confirmation Bias
Confirmation bias is a well-documented heuristic whereby people give more weight to information that fits with their existing worldviews or beliefs. At the same time, information that contradicts these beliefs is discounted or rejected.
Investors should be aware of their own tendency toward confirmation bias so that they can overcome poor decision-making, missing chances, and avoid falling prey to bubbles . Seeking out contrarian views and avoiding affirmative questions are two ways to counteract confirmation bias.
Hindsight Bias
Hindsight is always 20/20. However, the hindsight bias leads us to forget that we made incorrect predictions or estimates prior to them occurring. Rather, we become convinced that we had accurately predicted an event before it occurred, even when we did not. This can lead to overconfidence for making future predictions, or regret for not taking past opportunities.
Stereotypes
Stereotypes are a kind of heuristic that allows us to form opinions or judgments about people whom we have never met. In particular, stereotyping takes group-level characteristics about certain social groups—often ones that are racist, sexist, or otherwise discriminatory—and casts those characteristics onto all of the members in that group, regardless of their individual personalities, beliefs, skills, or behaviors.
By imposing oversimplified beliefs onto people, we can quickly judge potential interactions with them or individual outcomes of those people. However, these judgments are often plain wrong, derogatory, and perpetuate social divisions and exclusions.
Heuristics were first identified and taken seriously by scholars in the middle of the 20th century with the work of Herbert Simon, who asked why individuals and firms don't act like rational actors in the real world, even with market pressures punishing irrational decisions. Simon found that corporate managers do not usually optimize but instead rely on a set of heuristics or shortcuts to get the job done in a way that is good enough (to "satisfice").
Later, in the 1970s and '80s, psychologists Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman working at the Hebrew University in Jerusalem, built off of Herbert Simon's work and developed what is known as Prospect Theory . A cornerstone of behavioral economics, Prospect Theory catalogs several heuristics used subconsciously by people as they make financial evaluations.
One major finding is that people are loss-averse —that losses loom larger than gains (i.e., the pain of losing $50 is far more than the pleasure of receiving $50). Here, people adopt a heuristic to avoid realizing losses, sometimes spurring them to take excessive risks in order to do so—but often leading to even larger losses.
More recently, behavioral economists have tried to develop policy measures or "nudges" to help correct people's irrational use of heuristics in order to help them achieve more optimal outcomes—for instance, by having people enroll in a retirement savings plan by default instead of having to opt in.
What Are the Types of Heuristics?
To date, several heuristics have been identified by behavioral economics—or else developed to aid people in making otherwise complex decisions. In behavioral economics, representativeness, anchoring and adjustment, and availability (recency) are among the most widely cited. Heuristics may be categorized in many ways, such as cognitive versus emotional biases or errors in judgment versus errors in calculation.
What Is Heuristic Thinking?
Heuristic thinking uses mental shortcuts—often unconsciously—to quickly and efficiently make otherwise complex decisions or judgments. These can be in the form of a "rule of thumb" (e.g., saving 5% of your income in order to have a comfortable retirement) or cognitive processes that we are largely unaware of like the availability bias.
What Is Another Word for Heuristic?
Heuristic may also go by the following terms: rule of thumb; mental shortcut; educated guess; or satisfice.
How Does a Heuristic Differ From an Algorithm?
An algorithm is a step-by-step set of instructions that are followed to achieve some goal or outcome, often optimizing that outcome. They are formalized and can be expressed as a formula or "recipe." As such, they are reproducible in the sense that an algorithm will always provide the same output, given the same input.
A heuristic amounts to an educated guess or gut feeling. Rather than following a set of rules or instructions, a heuristic is a mental shortcut. Moreover, it often produces sub-optimal and even irrational outcomes that may differ even when given the same input.
What Are Computer Heuristics?
In computer science, a heuristic refers to a method of solving a problem that proves to be quicker or more efficient than traditional methods. This may involve using approximations rather than precise calculations or techniques that circumvent otherwise computationally intensive routines.
Heuristics are practical rules of thumb that manifest as mental shortcuts in judgment and decision-making. Without heuristics, our brains would not be able to function given the complexity of the world, the amount of data to process, and the calculative abilities required to form an optimal decision. Instead, heuristics allow us to make quick, good-enough choices.
However, these choices may also be subject to inaccuracies and systemic biases, such as those identified by behavioral economics.
Simon, Herbert. " Herbert Simon, Innovation, and Heuristics ." Mind & Society, vol. 17, 2019, pp. 97-109.
Kahneman, Daniel, and Tversky, Amos. " Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision Under Risk ." The Econometric Society, vol. 47, no. 2, 1979, pp. 263-292.
- Terms of Service
- Editorial Policy
- Privacy Policy
- Your Privacy Choices
- Bipolar Disorder
- Therapy Center
- When To See a Therapist
- Types of Therapy
- Best Online Therapy
- Best Couples Therapy
- Managing Stress
- Sleep and Dreaming
- Understanding Emotions
- Self-Improvement
- Healthy Relationships
- Student Resources
- Personality Types
- Guided Meditations
- Verywell Mind Insights
- 2024 Verywell Mind 25
- Mental Health in the Classroom
- Editorial Process
- Meet Our Review Board
- Crisis Support
What Are Heuristics?
These mental shortcuts lead to fast decisions—and biased thinking
Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."
Steven Gans, MD is board-certified in psychiatry and is an active supervisor, teacher, and mentor at Massachusetts General Hospital.
Verywell / Cindy Chung
- History and Origins
- Heuristics vs. Algorithms
- Heuristics and Bias
How to Make Better Decisions
If you need to make a quick decision, there's a good chance you'll rely on a heuristic to come up with a speedy solution. Heuristics are mental shortcuts that allow people to solve problems and make judgments quickly and efficiently. Common types of heuristics rely on availability, representativeness, familiarity, anchoring effects, mood, scarcity, and trial-and-error.
Think of these as mental "rule-of-thumb" strategies that shorten decision-making time. Such shortcuts allow us to function without constantly stopping to think about our next course of action.
However, heuristics have both benefits and drawbacks. These strategies can be handy in many situations but can also lead to cognitive biases . Becoming aware of this might help you make better and more accurate decisions.
Press Play for Advice On Making Decisions
Hosted by therapist Amy Morin, LCSW, this episode of The Verywell Mind Podcast shares a simple way to make a tough decision. Click below to listen now.
Follow Now : Apple Podcasts / Spotify / Google Podcasts
History of the Research on Heuristics
Nobel-prize winning economist and cognitive psychologist Herbert Simon originally introduced the concept of heuristics in psychology in the 1950s. He suggested that while people strive to make rational choices, human judgment is subject to cognitive limitations. Purely rational decisions would involve weighing every alternative's potential costs and possible benefits.
However, people are limited by the amount of time they have to make a choice and the amount of information they have at their disposal. Other factors, such as overall intelligence and accuracy of perceptions, also influence the decision-making process.
In the 1970s, psychologists Amos Tversky and Daniel Kahneman presented their research on cognitive biases. They proposed that these biases influence how people think and make judgments.
Because of these limitations, we must rely on mental shortcuts to help us make sense of the world.
Simon's research demonstrated that humans were limited in their ability to make rational decisions, but it was Tversky and Kahneman's work that introduced the study of heuristics and the specific ways of thinking that people rely on to simplify the decision-making process.
How Heuristics Are Used
Heuristics play important roles in both problem-solving and decision-making , as we often turn to these mental shortcuts when we need a quick solution.
Here are a few different theories from psychologists about why we rely on heuristics.
- Attribute substitution : People substitute simpler but related questions in place of more complex and difficult questions.
- Effort reduction : People use heuristics as a type of cognitive laziness to reduce the mental effort required to make choices and decisions.
- Fast and frugal : People use heuristics because they can be fast and correct in certain contexts. Some theories argue that heuristics are actually more accurate than they are biased.
In order to cope with the tremendous amount of information we encounter and to speed up the decision-making process, our brains rely on these mental strategies to simplify things so we don't have to spend endless amounts of time analyzing every detail.
You probably make hundreds or even thousands of decisions every day. What should you have for breakfast? What should you wear today? Should you drive or take the bus? Fortunately, heuristics allow you to make such decisions with relative ease and without a great deal of agonizing.
There are many heuristics examples in everyday life. When trying to decide if you should drive or ride the bus to work, for instance, you might remember that there is road construction along the bus route. You realize that this might slow the bus and cause you to be late for work. So you leave earlier and drive to work on an alternate route.
Heuristics allow you to think through the possible outcomes quickly and arrive at a solution.
Are Heuristics Good or Bad?
Heuristics aren't inherently good or bad, but there are pros and cons to using them to make decisions. While they can help us figure out a solution to a problem faster, they can also lead to inaccurate judgments about others or situations. Understanding these pros and cons may help you better use heuristics to make better decisions.
Types of Heuristics
There are many different kinds of heuristics. While each type plays a role in decision-making, they occur during different contexts. Understanding the types can help you better understand which one you are using and when.
Availability
The availability heuristic involves making decisions based upon how easy it is to bring something to mind. When you are trying to make a decision, you might quickly remember a number of relevant examples.
Since these are more readily available in your memory, you will likely judge these outcomes as being more common or frequently occurring.
For example, imagine you are planning to fly somewhere on vacation. As you are preparing for your trip, you might start to think of a number of recent airline accidents. You might feel like air travel is too dangerous and decide to travel by car instead. Because those examples of air disasters came to mind so easily, the availability heuristic leads you to think that plane crashes are more common than they really are.
Familiarity
The familiarity heuristic refers to how people tend to have more favorable opinions of things, people, or places they've experienced before as opposed to new ones. In fact, given two options, people may choose something they're more familiar with even if the new option provides more benefits.
Representativeness
The representativeness heuristic involves making a decision by comparing the present situation to the most representative mental prototype. When you are trying to decide if someone is trustworthy, you might compare aspects of the individual to other mental examples you hold.
A soft-spoken older woman might remind you of your grandmother, so you might immediately assume she is kind, gentle, and trustworthy. However, this is an example of a heuristic bias, as you can't know someone trustworthy based on their age alone.
The affect heuristic involves making choices that are influenced by an individual's emotions at that moment. For example, research has shown that people are more likely to see decisions as having benefits and lower risks when in a positive mood.
Negative emotions, on the other hand, lead people to focus on the potential downsides of a decision rather than the possible benefits.
The anchoring bias involves the tendency to be overly influenced by the first bit of information we hear or learn. This can make it more difficult to consider other factors and lead to poor choices. For example, anchoring bias can influence how much you are willing to pay for something, causing you to jump at the first offer without shopping around for a better deal.
Scarcity is a heuristic principle in which we view things that are scarce or less available to us as inherently more valuable. Marketers often use the scarcity heuristic to influence people to buy certain products. This is why you'll often see signs that advertise "limited time only," or that tell you to "get yours while supplies last."
Trial and Error
Trial and error is another type of heuristic in which people use a number of different strategies to solve something until they find what works. Examples of this type of heuristic are evident in everyday life.
People use trial and error when playing video games, finding the fastest driving route to work, or learning to ride a bike (or any new skill).
Difference Between Heuristics and Algorithms
Though the terms are often confused, heuristics and algorithms are two distinct terms in psychology.
Algorithms are step-by-step instructions that lead to predictable, reliable outcomes, whereas heuristics are mental shortcuts that are basically best guesses. Algorithms always lead to accurate outcomes, whereas, heuristics do not.
Examples of algorithms include instructions for how to put together a piece of furniture or a recipe for cooking a certain dish. Health professionals also create algorithms or processes to follow in order to determine what type of treatment to use on a patient.
How Heuristics Can Lead to Bias
Heuristics can certainly help us solve problems and speed up our decision-making process, but that doesn't mean they are always a good thing. They can also introduce errors, bias, and irrational decision-making. As in the examples above, heuristics can lead to inaccurate judgments about how commonly things occur and how representative certain things may be.
Just because something has worked in the past does not mean that it will work again, and relying on a heuristic can make it difficult to see alternative solutions or come up with new ideas.
Heuristics can also contribute to stereotypes and prejudice . Because people use mental shortcuts to classify and categorize people, they often overlook more relevant information and create stereotyped categorizations that are not in tune with reality.
While heuristics can be a useful tool, there are ways you can improve your decision-making and avoid cognitive bias at the same time.
We are more likely to make an error in judgment if we are trying to make a decision quickly or are under pressure to do so. Taking a little more time to make a decision can help you see things more clearly—and make better choices.
Whenever possible, take a few deep breaths and do something to distract yourself from the decision at hand. When you return to it, you may find a fresh perspective or notice something you didn't before.
Identify the Goal
We tend to focus automatically on what works for us and make decisions that serve our best interest. But take a moment to know what you're trying to achieve. Consider some of the following questions:
- Are there other people who will be affected by this decision?
- What's best for them?
- Is there a common goal that can be achieved that will serve all parties?
Thinking through these questions can help you figure out your goals and the impact that these decisions may have.
Process Your Emotions
Fast decision-making is often influenced by emotions from past experiences that bubble to the surface. Anger, sadness, love, and other powerful feelings can sometimes lead us to decisions we might not otherwise make.
Is your decision based on facts or emotions? While emotions can be helpful, they may affect decisions in a negative way if they prevent us from seeing the full picture.
Recognize All-or-Nothing Thinking
When making a decision, it's a common tendency to believe you have to pick a single, well-defined path, and there's no going back. In reality, this often isn't the case.
Sometimes there are compromises involving two choices, or a third or fourth option that we didn't even think of at first. Try to recognize the nuances and possibilities of all choices involved, instead of using all-or-nothing thinking .
Heuristics are common and often useful. We need this type of decision-making strategy to help reduce cognitive load and speed up many of the small, everyday choices we must make as we live, work, and interact with others.
But it pays to remember that heuristics can also be flawed and lead to irrational choices if we rely too heavily on them. If you are making a big decision, give yourself a little extra time to consider your options and try to consider the situation from someone else's perspective. Thinking things through a bit instead of relying on your mental shortcuts can help ensure you're making the right choice.
Vlaev I. Local choices: Rationality and the contextuality of decision-making . Brain Sci . 2018;8(1):8. doi:10.3390/brainsci8010008
Hjeij M, Vilks A. A brief history of heuristics: how did research on heuristics evolve? Humanit Soc Sci Commun . 2023;10(1):64. doi:10.1057/s41599-023-01542-z
Brighton H, Gigerenzer G. Homo heuristicus: Less-is-more effects in adaptive cognition . Malays J Med Sci . 2012;19(4):6-16.
Schwartz PH. Comparative risk: Good or bad heuristic? Am J Bioeth . 2016;16(5):20-22. doi:10.1080/15265161.2016.1159765
Schwikert SR, Curran T. Familiarity and recollection in heuristic decision making . J Exp Psychol Gen . 2014;143(6):2341-2365. doi:10.1037/xge0000024
AlKhars M, Evangelopoulos N, Pavur R, Kulkarni S. Cognitive biases resulting from the representativeness heuristic in operations management: an experimental investigation . Psychol Res Behav Manag . 2019;12:263-276. doi:10.2147/PRBM.S193092
Finucane M, Alhakami A, Slovic P, Johnson S. The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits . J Behav Decis Mak . 2000; 13(1):1-17. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(200001/03)13:1<1::AID-BDM333>3.0.CO;2-S
Teovanović P. Individual differences in anchoring effect: Evidence for the role of insufficient adjustment . Eur J Psychol . 2019;15(1):8-24. doi:10.5964/ejop.v15i1.1691
Cheung TT, Kroese FM, Fennis BM, De Ridder DT. Put a limit on it: The protective effects of scarcity heuristics when self-control is low . Health Psychol Open . 2015;2(2):2055102915615046. doi:10.1177/2055102915615046
Mohr H, Zwosta K, Markovic D, Bitzer S, Wolfensteller U, Ruge H. Deterministic response strategies in a trial-and-error learning task . Inman C, ed. PLoS Comput Biol. 2018;14(11):e1006621. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1006621
Grote T, Berens P. On the ethics of algorithmic decision-making in healthcare . J Med Ethics . 2020;46(3):205-211. doi:10.1136/medethics-2019-105586
Bigler RS, Clark C. The inherence heuristic: A key theoretical addition to understanding social stereotyping and prejudice. Behav Brain Sci . 2014;37(5):483-4. doi:10.1017/S0140525X1300366X
del Campo C, Pauser S, Steiner E, et al. Decision making styles and the use of heuristics in decision making . J Bus Econ. 2016;86:389–412. doi:10.1007/s11573-016-0811-y
Marewski JN, Gigerenzer G. Heuristic decision making in medicine . Dialogues Clin Neurosci . 2012;14(1):77-89. doi:10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.1/jmarewski
Zheng Y, Yang Z, Jin C, Qi Y, Liu X. The influence of emotion on fairness-related decision making: A critical review of theories and evidence . Front Psychol . 2017;8:1592. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01592
By Kendra Cherry, MSEd Kendra Cherry, MS, is a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, psychology educator, and author of the "Everything Psychology Book."
Grab Your Free Copy of The State of Leadership Development Report 2024
Heuristic Problem Solving: A comprehensive guide with 5 Examples
What are heuristics, advantages of using heuristic problem solving, disadvantages of using heuristic problem solving, heuristic problem solving examples, frequently asked questions.
- Speed: Heuristics are designed to find solutions quickly, saving time in problem solving tasks. Rather than spending a lot of time analyzing every possible solution, heuristics help to narrow down the options and focus on the most promising ones.
- Flexibility: Heuristics are not rigid, step-by-step procedures. They allow for flexibility and creativity in problem solving, leading to innovative solutions. They encourage thinking outside the box and can generate unexpected and valuable ideas.
- Simplicity: Heuristics are often easy to understand and apply, making them accessible to anyone regardless of their expertise or background. They don’t require specialized knowledge or training, which means they can be used in various contexts and by different people.
- Cost-effective: Because heuristics are simple and efficient, they can save time, money, and effort in finding solutions. They also don’t require expensive software or equipment, making them a cost-effective approach to problem solving.
- Real-world applicability: Heuristics are often based on practical experience and knowledge, making them relevant to real-world situations. They can help solve complex, messy, or ill-defined problems where other problem solving methods may not be practical.
- Potential for errors: Heuristic problem solving relies on generalizations and assumptions, which may lead to errors or incorrect conclusions. This is especially true if the heuristic is not based on a solid understanding of the problem or the underlying principles.
- Limited scope: Heuristic problem solving may only consider a limited number of potential solutions and may not identify the most optimal or effective solution.
- Lack of creativity: Heuristic problem solving may rely on pre-existing solutions or approaches, limiting creativity and innovation in problem-solving.
- Over-reliance: Heuristic problem solving may lead to over-reliance on a specific approach or heuristic, which can be problematic if the heuristic is flawed or ineffective.
- Lack of transparency: Heuristic problem solving may not be transparent or explainable, as the decision-making process may not be explicitly articulated or understood.
- Trial and error: This heuristic involves trying different solutions to a problem and learning from mistakes until a successful solution is found. A software developer encountering a bug in their code may try other solutions and test each one until they find the one that solves the issue.
- Working backward: This heuristic involves starting at the goal and then figuring out what steps are needed to reach that goal. For example, a project manager may begin by setting a project deadline and then work backward to determine the necessary steps and deadlines for each team member to ensure the project is completed on time.
- Breaking a problem into smaller parts: This heuristic involves breaking down a complex problem into smaller, more manageable pieces that can be tackled individually. For example, an HR manager tasked with implementing a new employee benefits program may break the project into smaller parts, such as researching options, getting quotes from vendors, and communicating the unique benefits to employees.
- Using analogies: This heuristic involves finding similarities between a current problem and a similar problem that has been solved before and using the solution to the previous issue to help solve the current one. For example, a salesperson struggling to close a deal may use an analogy to a successful sales pitch they made to help guide their approach to the current pitch.
- Simplifying the problem: This heuristic involves simplifying a complex problem by ignoring details that are not necessary for solving it. This allows the problem solver to focus on the most critical aspects of the problem. For example, a customer service representative dealing with a complex issue may simplify it by breaking it down into smaller components and addressing them individually rather than simultaneously trying to solve the entire problem.
Test your problem-solving skills for free in just a few minutes.
The free problem-solving skills for managers and team leaders helps you understand mistakes that hold you back.
What are the three types of heuristics?
What are the four stages of heuristics in problem solving.
Other Related Blogs
Top 15 Tips for Effective Conflict Mediation at Work
Top 10 games for negotiation skills to make you a better leader, manager effectiveness: a complete guide for managers in 2024, 5 proven ways managers can build collaboration in a team.
The Pros and Cons of Heuristic Evaluation
Heuristics are a set of established, empirical norms or principles that are applied to a discipline or process. They are derived from observation, research and experience over a wide range of projects and usually over a long period of time so they have some cogency and credibility in the field to which they apply.
Heuristic evaluation is a process applied to a project or product that employs these principles to assess how the product performs against a clearly defined set of criteria.
It is conducted by experts in the field who test and review the product with the criteria at the forefront of the process. While this is undoubtedly a useful approach it does have its drawbacks and critics and it is very dependent on a number of value judgements and inputs that may or may not be accurate. Here is a brief look at the pros and cons of heuristic evaluation.
- It is a detailed, technically sound process that assesses the product against very clear criteria.
- Because it is done by several people there is a better chance of getting a range of views and picking up more potential problem areas.
- The very act of setting up the heuristic evaluation is a useful exercise as it forces you to identify the root elements of the product and focuses development on the main issues.
- There are fewer practical and ethical issues attached to heuristic evaluation as testers are testing in a virtual space.
- Heuristic evaluation tends to focus on fewer, more relevant areas so the problems it identifies tend to be important ones.
- The evaluation is only as good as the people you get to do it. This means you have to spend a lot of time analysing and reviewing experts to make sure they are relevant and experienced in the issues you are concerned with.
- Another disadvantages of heuristics is that a number of experts are required and this can be time-consuming and expensive to research and set up.
- You are getting opinions and personal observation rather than hard, empirical data from the exercise and the experts’ own background, attitudes, and preferences might colour the verdicts.
- You have to do a good deal of analysis and thinking to make sure you choose the right heuristics in the first place. If this is wrong, no matter how good the experts are, you are likely to get less than optimum results.
- Often the problems identified are not critical (or even real in some cases).
Heuristic evaluations are certainly useful in some instances and can provide crucial insights into how your site is meeting its objectives without the time, expense and potential problems of real user evaluation. It can. However, be risky to rely on it as the sole means of testing your concept and product.
If you would like more information on heuristic evaluation (or any other type of user evaluation) why not ring us on +44(0)800 0246247 or email us at [email protected]
Related Posts
Comments (1)
[…] Advantage Of Heuristic Method […]
Comments are closed.
Sign-Up To Our UX Newsletter !
The ‘UX Crucible’ is a monthly summary of user experience news and stories from around the world.
Subscribe now using the form below.
- I consent to having this website store my submitted information so they can subscribe me to the newsletter.
Reviewed by Psychology Today Staff
A heuristic is a mental shortcut that allows an individual to make a decision, pass judgment, or solve a problem quickly and with minimal mental effort. While heuristics can reduce the burden of decision-making and free up limited cognitive resources, they can also be costly when they lead individuals to miss critical information or act on unjust biases.
- Understanding Heuristics
- Different Heuristics
- Problems with Heuristics
As humans move throughout the world, they must process large amounts of information and make many choices with limited amounts of time. When information is missing, or an immediate decision is necessary, heuristics act as “rules of thumb” that guide behavior down the most efficient pathway.
Heuristics are not unique to humans; animals use heuristics that, though less complex, also serve to simplify decision-making and reduce cognitive load.
Generally, yes. Navigating day-to-day life requires everyone to make countless small decisions within a limited timeframe. Heuristics can help individuals save time and mental energy, freeing up cognitive resources for more complex planning and problem-solving endeavors.
The human brain and all its processes—including heuristics— developed over millions of years of evolution . Since mental shortcuts save both cognitive energy and time, they likely provided an advantage to those who relied on them.
Heuristics that were helpful to early humans may not be universally beneficial today . The familiarity heuristic, for example—in which the familiar is preferred over the unknown—could steer early humans toward foods or people that were safe, but may trigger anxiety or unfair biases in modern times.
The study of heuristics was developed by renowned psychologists Daniel Kahneman and Amos Tversky. Starting in the 1970s, Kahneman and Tversky identified several different kinds of heuristics, most notably the availability heuristic and the anchoring heuristic.
Since then, researchers have continued their work and identified many different kinds of heuristics, including:
Familiarity heuristic
Fundamental attribution error
Representativeness heuristic
Satisficing
The anchoring heuristic, or anchoring bias , occurs when someone relies more heavily on the first piece of information learned when making a choice, even if it's not the most relevant. In such cases, anchoring is likely to steer individuals wrong .
The availability heuristic describes the mental shortcut in which someone estimates whether something is likely to occur based on how readily examples come to mind . People tend to overestimate the probability of plane crashes, homicides, and shark attacks, for instance, because examples of such events are easily remembered.
People who make use of the representativeness heuristic categorize objects (or other people) based on how similar they are to known entities —assuming someone described as "quiet" is more likely to be a librarian than a politician, for instance.
Satisficing is a decision-making strategy in which the first option that satisfies certain criteria is selected , even if other, better options may exist.
Heuristics, while useful, are imperfect; if relied on too heavily, they can result in incorrect judgments or cognitive biases. Some are more likely to steer people wrong than others.
Assuming, for example, that child abductions are common because they’re frequently reported on the news—an example of the availability heuristic—may trigger unnecessary fear or overprotective parenting practices. Understanding commonly unhelpful heuristics, and identifying situations where they could affect behavior, may help individuals avoid such mental pitfalls.
Sometimes called the attribution effect or correspondence bias, the term describes a tendency to attribute others’ behavior primarily to internal factors—like personality or character— while attributing one’s own behavior more to external or situational factors .
If one person steps on the foot of another in a crowded elevator, the victim may attribute it to carelessness. If, on the other hand, they themselves step on another’s foot, they may be more likely to attribute the mistake to being jostled by someone else .
Listen to your gut, but don’t rely on it . Think through major problems methodically—by making a list of pros and cons, for instance, or consulting with people you trust. Make extra time to think through tasks where snap decisions could cause significant problems, such as catching an important flight.
Many apps claim to use nudges but instead use simple reminders that are just re-labeled as nudges. The terminology matters—true nudges use more potent behavioral science.
When you make a mistake for the third time, it's probably about you, not the situation.
The myth of the perfect question, the "fit" fallacy, and more.
Your plans should not be set based on the wisdom shared by successful people. They should be based on your context, your strengths, and your creative inspiration from that wisdom.
Discover how the anchoring effect, a subtle cognitive bias, shapes our decisions across life's domains.
Discrimination is how we treat groups of people. Bias is how we think about them. Can better understanding cognitive biases make a difference in the fight against discrimination?
Artificial intelligence already plays a role in deciding who’s getting hired. The way to improve AI is very similar to how we fight human biases.
Think you are avoiding the motherhood penalty by not having children? Think again. Simply being a woman of childbearing age can trigger discrimination.
Psychological experiments on human judgment under uncertainty showed that people often stray from presumptions about rational economic agents.
Are experts more confident in what they know than what they don't? Yes, but it's not so clear-cut.
- Find a Therapist
- Find a Treatment Center
- Find a Psychiatrist
- Find a Support Group
- Find Online Therapy
- United States
- Brooklyn, NY
- Chicago, IL
- Houston, TX
- Los Angeles, CA
- New York, NY
- Portland, OR
- San Diego, CA
- San Francisco, CA
- Seattle, WA
- Washington, DC
- Asperger's
- Bipolar Disorder
- Chronic Pain
- Eating Disorders
- Passive Aggression
- Personality
- Goal Setting
- Positive Psychology
- Stopping Smoking
- Low Sexual Desire
- Relationships
- Child Development
- Self Tests NEW
- Therapy Center
- Diagnosis Dictionary
- Types of Therapy
When we fall prey to perfectionism, we think we’re honorably aspiring to be our very best, but often we’re really just setting ourselves up for failure, as perfection is impossible and its pursuit inevitably backfires.
- Emotional Intelligence
- Gaslighting
- Affective Forecasting
- Neuroscience
An Overview of Heuristics and Metaheuristics
- First Online: 08 July 2022
Cite this chapter
- Saïd Salhi 3 &
- Jonathan Thompson ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-3384-5240 4
1977 Accesses
3 Citations
There are numerous optimisation problems for which heuristics are currently the only practical solution method. This chapter explains the need for heuristics before providing a brief overview of heuristic search techniques including metaheuristics. These include traditional local search methods including simulated annealing and tabu search, population methods including genetic algorithms and ant colony optimisation and more recently proposed methods such as harmony search and particle swarm optimisation. Examples are provided from successful applications and we will also discuss potential future research avenues.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this chapter
Subscribe and save.
- Get 10 units per month
- Download Article/Chapter or eBook
- 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
- Cancel anytime
- Available as PDF
- Read on any device
- Instant download
- Own it forever
- Available as EPUB and PDF
- Compact, lightweight edition
- Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
- Free shipping worldwide - see info
- Durable hardcover edition
Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout
Purchases are for personal use only
Institutional subscriptions
Similar content being viewed by others
Introduction: Optimization and Metaheuristics Algorithms
Metaheuristic Optimization: Nature-Inspired Algorithms and Applications
Metaheuristics: a comprehensive overview and classification along with bibliometric analysis
Ahuja RK, Ergun O, Orlin JB and Punnen AP (2002). A survey of very large scale neighbourhood search techniques. Discrete Appl Math 123: 75–102.
Article Google Scholar
Aickelin U and Dowsland K (2000). Exploiting problem structure in a genetic algorithm approach to a nurse rostering problem. J Sched 3: 139–153.
Basir N, Ismail W and Norwawi, N (2013). A simulated annealing for Tahmidi course timetabling. Procedia Technology 11: 437–445.
Battiti R and Tecchiolli G (1994). The reactive tabu search. ORSA J Comput 6: 126–140.
Bertsimas D, Cacchiani V, Craft D and Nohadani O (2013). A hybrid approach to beam angle optimization in intensity-modulated radiation therapy. Comput Oper Res 40: 2187–2197.
Borchani E, Elloumi A and Masmoudi M (2017). Variable neighbourhood descent search algorithms for course timetabling problem: Application to a Tunisian University. Electronic Notes in Discrete Math. 58: 119–126.
Brandao J and Mercer A (1997). A tabu search heuristic for the multiple-trip vehicle routing and scheduling problem. Eur J Oper Res 100: 180–191.
Braysy O, Hasle G and Dullaert W (2004). A multi-start local search algorithm for the vehicle routing problem with time windows. Eur J Oper Res 159 (3): 586–605.
Google Scholar
Bullnheimer B, Harlt R and Strauss C (1998). Applying ant systems to the vehicle routing problem. In Voss S, Martello S, Osman IH and Roucairal C (eds), Metaheuristics: Advances and Trends in Local Search Paradigms for Optimization. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston.
Burke EK, De Causmaecker P, Berghe GV and Van Landeghem H (2004). The state of the art of nurse rostering. J Sched 7: 441–499.
Burke EK, Curtois T, Post G, Qu R and Veltman B (2008). A hybrid heuristic ordering and variable neighbourhood search for the nurse rostering problem. Eur J Oper Res 188 (2): 330–341.
Cerny V (1982). A thermodynamical approach to the travelling salesman problem: an efficient simulation algorithm. J Optim Theory Appl 45: 41–51.
Charon I and Hudry O (1993). The noising method—a new method for combinatorial optimization. Oper Res Let 14: 133–137.
Charon I and Hudry O (2009). Self-tuning of the noising method. Optimization 58: 1–21.
Colorni A, Dorigo M and Maniezzo V (1991). Distributed optimization by ant colonies. In Varela F and Bourgine P (eds) Proceedings of the European Conference on Artificial Life. Elsevier Publishing, Amsterdam, 457–474.
Conolly DT (1990). An improved simulated annealing technique for the QAP. Eur J Oper Res 46: 93–100.
Conolly D (1992). General purpose simulated annealing. J Opl Res Soc 43: 495–505.
Corne D, Ross P and Fang H-L (2005). Fast practical evolutionary timetabling. In: Fogarty T C (ed) Evolutionary Computing. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 865, Srpinger, Berlin, Heidelberg.
Costa D (1995). An evolutionary tabu search algorithm and the NHL scheduling problem. INFOR 33: 161–178.
Cunha CB and Silva ME (2007). A genetic algorithm for the problem of configuring a hub-and-spoke network for a LTL trucking company in Brazil. Eur J Oper Res 179: 747–758.
Daneubourg JL, Aron A, Goss S and Pasteels JM (1990). The self organising exploratory pattern of the argentine ant. J Insec Behav 3: 159–168.
Dasgupta D and Michalewicz Z (Eds) (2013). Evolutionary Algorithms in Engineering Applications. Springer, New York.
Dasgupta D (Ed) (1999). Artificial Immune System and Their Applications. Springer- Verlag.
Dias J, Rocha H, Ferreira B, de Carmo Lopes C (2014). A genetic algorithm with neural network fitness function evaluation for IMRT beam angle optimization. Cent Eur J Oper Res 22: 431–455.
Dias J, Rocha H, Ferreira B, de Carmo Lopes C (2014). Simulated annealing applied to IMRT beam angle optimization: a computational study. Physica Medica 31: 747–756.
Di Gaspero L and Schaerf A (2001). Tabu search techniques for examination timetabling. In: EK Burke and W Erben (eds) Selected Papers from the Third International Conference on the Practice and Theory of Automated Timetabling. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 2079, 104–117.
Dorigo M and Gambardella LM (1997). Ant colony system: a cooperative learning approach to the travelling salesman problem. IEEE Trans Evol Comput 1: 53–66.
Dorigi M and Stutzle T (2010). Ant colony optimization: overview and recent advances. In Gendreau M and Potvin JY (eds) Handbook of Metaheuristics (2nd edition). Springer, London, pp 227–264.
Dorigo M, Caro G and Gambardella L (1999). Ant algorithms for discrete optimization. Art Life 5: 137–172.
Dowsland KA (1993). Some experiments with simulated annealing techniques for packing problems. Eur J Oper Res 68: 389–399.
Dowsland KA and Thompson JM (1998). A robust simulated annealing based examination timetabling system. Comp Oper Res 25: 637–648.
Dowsland KA and Thompson JM (2000). Solving a nurse scheduling problem with knapsacks, network and tabu search. J Oper Res Soc 51: 825–833.
Dowsland KA and Thompson JM (2012). Simulated annealing. In Rozenberg G, Back T and Kok JN (eds) Handbook of Natural Computing. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp 1624–1655.
Drezner Z and Salhi S (2002). Using hybrid metaheuristics for the one-way and two-way network design problem. Nav Res Logistics 49: 449–463.
Dueck G and Scheuer T (1990). Threshold accepting: a general purpose optimization algorithm superior to simulated annealing. J Comput Phy 90: 161–175.
Dueck G (1993). New optimization heuristics: the great deluge algorithm and the record-to-record travel. J Comp Phys 104: 86–92.
Eglese R (1990). Simulated annealing: a tool for operational research. Eur J Oper Res 46: 271–281.
Elshaikh A, Salhi S, Brimberg J, Mladenović N, Callaghan B and Nagy G (2016). An adaptive perturbation-based heuristic: an application to the continuous p-centre problem. Comput Oper Res 75: 1–11.
Feo TA and Resende MGC (1989). A probablistic heuristic for a computationally difficult set covering problem. Opns Res Lett 8: 67–71.
Feo TA and Resende MGC (1995). Greedy randomized adaptive search procedures. J Glob Opt 6: 109–133.
Fletcher R (1989). Practical Methods of Optimisation. John Wiley and Sons, New York.
Geem ZW, Kim JH and Loganathan GV (2001). A new heuristic optimization algorithm: harmony search. Simulation 76 (2): 60–68.
Glover F (1986). Future paths for integer programming and links to artificial intelligence. Comput Opns Res 13: 533–549.
Glover F, Laguna M and Marti R (2003) Scatter search and path relinking: advances and applications. In Glover F and Kochenberger GA (eds) Handbook of Metaheuristics. Kluwer Academic Publisher, London, pp 1–35.
Chapter Google Scholar
Goldberg DE (1989). Genetic Algorithm in Search, Optimization and Machine Learning. Addison-Wesley, New York.
Goodman M, Dowsland KA and Thompson JM (2009) A grasp-knapsack hybrid for a nurse-scheduling problem. J Heuristics 15: 351–379.
Hansen P (1986). The steepest ascent, mildest descent heuristic for combinatorial programming. Paper presented at the congress on Numerical Methods in Combinatorial Optimization, Capri, Italy.
Hansen P, Mladenović N, Brimberg J and Moreno Perez JA (2019). Variable neighbourhood search. In Gendreau M and Potvin JY (eds) Handbook of metaheuristics (latest edition). Springer, Cham, pp 57–97.
Hansen P, Mladenović N, Todosijević and Hanafi S (2017). Variable neighborhood search: basics and variants. EURO J Comput Optim 5: 423–454.
Holland JH (1975). Adaptation in Natural and Artificial Systems. University of Michigan Press, Ann Harbor.
Hemmelmayr V, Doerner KF, Hartl RF and Savelsbergh MWP (2009). Delivery strategies for blood products supplies. OR Spec 31: 707–725.
Hu TC, Kahng AB and Tsao CWA (1995). Old bachelor acceptance: a new class of non-monotone threshold accepting methods. ORSA J Comput 7: 417–425.
James C and Salhi S (2000). The location of protection devices on electrical tree networks: a heuristic approach. J Oper Res Soc 51: 959–970.
James C and Salhi S (2000). A tabu search heuristic for the location of multi type protection devices on electrical tree networks. J Com Opt 6: 81–98.
Kadu MS, Gupta R and Bhave P (2008). Optimal design of water networks using a modified genetic algorithm with reduction in search space. J Water Res Plan Manage 134: 147–160.
Karaboga D and Basturk B (2007). Artificial bee colony optimization algorithm for solving constrained optimization problems. In Melin P, Castillo O, Aguilar L, Kacprzyk J and Pedrycz, W (eds) Foundations of Fuzzy Logic and Soft Computing. Lecture Notes in Computer Science 4529, Berlin, Heidelberg.
Kendall G (2008). Scheduling English football fixtures over holiday periods. J Oper Res Soc 59: 743–755.
Kennedy J and Eberhault RC (1995). Particle Swarm Optimization. IEEE Int Conf Neural Networks, Perth, Australia, pp 1942–1948.
Kirkpatrick S, Gelat CD and Vecchi MP (1983). Optimization by simulated annealing. Science 220: 671–680.
Kumar DN and Reddy MJ (2006). Ant colony optimization for multi-purpose reservoir operation. Water Res Manage 20: 879–898.
Kumar DN and Reddy MJ (2007). Multi-purpose reservoir operation using particle swarm optimization. J Water Resour Plann Manag 133 (3): 192–201.
Laarhoven PJM and Aarts EHL (1987). Simulated Annealing: Theory and Applications. Reidel, Rotterdam.
Laporte G, Gendreau M, Potvin J-Y and Semet F (2000). Classical and modern heuristics for the vehicle routing problem. International Transaction in Operational Research 7: 285–300.
Lee DS, Vassiliadis VS and Park JM (2004). A novel threshold accepting meta-heuristic for the job-shop scheduling problem. Comp Oper Res 31 (13): 2199–2213.
Lewis R (2008). A survey of meta-heuristic based techniques for university timetabling problems. OR Spektrum 30: 167–190.
Li F, Golden B and Wasil E (2007). A record-to-record travel algorithm for solving the heterogeneous fleet vehicle routing problem. Comput Opns Res 34: 2734–2742.
Lourenco HR, Martin OC and Stutzle T (2010). Iterated local search: framework and applications. In Gendreau M and Potvin JY (eds) Handbook of Metaheuristics. Springer, London, pp 363–397.
Lu C (2013). Robust weighted vertex p-center model considering uncertain data: an application to emergency management. Eur J Oper Res 230: 113–121.
Lundy M and Mees A (1986). Convergence of an annealing algorithm. Math Prog 34: 111–124.
Luis M, Salhi S and Nagy G (2011). A guided reactive GRASP for the capacitated multi-source Weber problem. Comp Oper Res 38 (7): 1014–1024.
Mahdavi M, Fesanghary M and Damangir E (2007). An improved harmony search algorithm for solving optimization problems. Applied Mathematics and Computation 188: 1567–1579.
Marti R, Laguna M and Glover F (2006). Principles of scatter search. Eur J Oper Res 169: 359–372.
Maslow AH (1954). Motivation and Personality. Harper & Sons, New York.
Metropolis N, Rosenbluth A, Rosenbluth M, Teller A and Teller E (1953). Equations of state calculations by fast computing machines. J Chem Phy 21: 1087–1092.
Mladenović N and Hansen P (1997). Variable neighbourhood search. Comput Oper Res 24: 1097–1100.
Osman IH and Christofides N (1994). Capacitated clustering problems by hybrid simulated annealing and tabu search. Int Trans Oper Res 1: 317–336.
Mwenbeshi MM, Kent CA and Salhi S (2004). A genetic algorithm based approach to intelligent modelling and control of pH in reactors. Comp Chem Eng 28 (9): 1743–1757.
Osman IH and Laporte G (1996). Metaheuristics: a bibliography. Ann Oper Res 63: 513–623.
Pacheto JA and Casado S (2004). Solving two location models with few facilities by using a hybrid heuristic: a real health resources case. Comput Oper Res 32: 3075–3091.
Pan QK, Suganthan PN, Tasgetiren MF and Liang JJ (2010). A self-adaptive global best harmony search algorithm for continuous optimization problems. Appl Math Comput 216: 830–848.
Pham DT, Ghanbarzadeh A, Koc E, Otri S, Rahim S and Zaidi M (2006). The bees algorithm, a novel tool for complex optimisation problems. In Proc 2nd Virtual International Conference on Intelligent Production Machines and Systems, Elsevier, Oxford, pp 454–459.
Pillay N and Banzhaf W (2010). An informed genetic algorithm for the examination problem. Appl Soft Comput 10 (2): 457–467.
Rochat Y and Semet F (1994). A tabu search approach for delivering pet food and flour in Switzerland. J Oper Res Soc 45: 1233–1246.
Rubinstein RY (1997). Optimization of computer simulation models with rare events. Eur J Oper Res 99: 89–112.
Rubinstein RY and Kroese DP (2004). The cross-entropy method: a unified approach to combinatorial optimization, Monte Carlo simulation and machine learning. Springer-Verlag, New York.
Salhi S (1997). A perturbation heuristic for a class of location problem. J Oper Res Soc 48: 1233–1240.
Salhi S (1998). Heuristic search methods. In Marcoulides GA (ed) Modern methods for business research. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey, pp 147–175.
Salhi S (2002). Defining tabu list size and aspiration criterion within tabu search methods. Comput Opns Res 29: 67–86.
Salhi S and Rand GK (1987). Improvements to vehicle routing heuristics. J Oper Res Soc 38: 293–295.
Salhi S and Sari M (1997). A Multi-level composite heuristic for the multi-depot vehicle fleet mix problem. Eur J Oper Res 103: 78–95.
Salhi S and Gamal MDH (2003). A genetic algorithm based approach for the uncapacitated continuous location–allocation problem. Ann Oper Res 123: 203–222.
Salhi S (2006). Heuristic search in action: the science of tomorrow. In Salhi S (Ed) OR48 Keynote papers. ORS Bath, pp 39–58.
Salhi S (2017). Heuristic Search: The Emerging Science of Problem Solving. Palgrave MacMillan.
Salhi S, Gutierrez, Wassan N, Wu S and Kaya R (2020). An effective real time GRASP-based metaheuristic: application to order consolidation and dynamic selection of transshipment points for time-critical freight logistics. Expert Syst Appl 158: 113574.
Salhi S and Petch R (2007). A GA based heuristic for the vehicle routing problem with multiple trips. J Math Model Algor 6: 591–613.
Salhi S, Imran A and Wassan NA (2014). The multi-depot vehicle routing problem with heterogeneous vehicle fleet: formulation and a variable neighbourhood search implementation. Comput Oprs Res 52: 315–325.
Salhi S and Thompson J (2021). The new era of hybridisation and learning in heuristic search design. In Salhi S and Boylan J (eds) The Palgrave Handbook of Operations Research. Palgrave, London.
Schrimpf G, Schneider J, Stamm-Wilbrabdt H and Dueck H (2000). Record breaking optimization results-using the ruin and recreate principle. J Comput Phys 159: 139–171.
Semet F and Taillard E (1993). Solving real-life vehicle routing problems efficiently using tabu search. Ann Oper Res 41: 469–488.
Shaw P (1998). Using constrain programming and local search methods to solve vehicle routing problem. In Proceeding CP-98 (Fourth International Conference on Principles and Practice of Constraint Programming).
Sorenson K, Sevaux M and Glover F (2018). A history of metaheuristics. In Marti R, Pardalos P and Resende M (eds) Handbook of Heuristics. Springer, 791–808.
Stützle T and Hoos HH (2000). MAX–MIN ant system. Futur Gener Comput Syst 16: 889–914.
Sze J, Salhi S and Wassan N (2016). A hybridisation of adaptive variable neighbourhood search and large neighbourhood search: application to the vehicle routing problem. Expert Syst Appl 65: 383–397.
Sze J, Salhi S and Wassan N (2017). The cumulative capacitated vehicle routing problem with min-sum and min-max objectives: an effective hybridisation of adaptive variable neighbourhood search and large neighbourhood search. Transp Res Part B 101: 162–184.
Szeto WY, Wu Y and Ho SC (2011). An artificial bee colony algorithm for the capacitated vehicle routing problem. Eur J Oper Res 215: 126–135.
Tarantilis CD and Kiranoudis CT (2002). BoneRoute: an adaptive memory-based method for effective fleet management. Ann Opns Res 115: 227–241.
Tarantilis CD, Kiranoudis C and Vassiliadis V (2003). A list based threshold accepting metahauristic for the heterogeneous fixed vehicle routing problem. J Oper Res Soc 54: 65–71.
Tarantilis CD and Kiranoudis CT (2007). A flexible adaptive memory-based algorithm for real-life transportation operations: two case studies from Diary and construction sector. Eur J Oper Res 179: 806–822.
Thompson J (1999). Kicking timetabling problems into touch. OR Insight 12: 7–15.
Tiwari MK, Prakash A, Kumar A, Mileham AR (2005). Determination of an optimal sequence using the psychoclonal algorithm. J Eng Manuf 219: 137–149.
Valouxis G, Gogos C, Goulas G and Alefragis P (2012). A systematic two phase approach for the nurse rostering problem. Eur J Oper Res 219: 425–433.
Voudouris C and Tsang EPK (2010). Guided local search. In Gendreau M, and Potvin JY (eds) Handbook of Metaheuristcs. Springer, London, pp 321–361.
Wade AC and Salhi S (2003). An ant system algorithm for the mixed vehicle routing problem with backhauls. In Resende MG and de Sousa JP (eds) Metaheuristics: Computer Decision-Making. Kluwer, NY, pp 699–719.
Wang H, Yao Y and Salhi S (2021). Tension in big data using machine learning: analysis and applications. Tech For Soc Change 158: 120175.
Wassan NA (2006). A reactive tabu search for vehicle routing. J Oper Res Soc 57: 111–116.
Willis R and Terrill B (1994). Scheduling the Australian state cricket season using simulated annealing. J Oper Res Soc 45: 276–280.
Wright M (1994). Timetabling county cricket fixtures using a form of tabu search. J Oper Res Soc 45: 758–770.
Wright M (1996). School timetabling using heuristic search. J Oper Res Soc 47: 347–357.
Wright M (2005). Scheduling fixtures for New Zealand cricket. IMA J Manag Math 16: 99–112.
Zainuddin ZM and Salhi S (2007). A perturbation-based heuristic for the capacitated multisource Weber problem. Eur J Oper Res 179: 1194–1207.
Download references
Author information
Authors and affiliations.
Kent Business School, University of Kent, Canterbury, UK
School of Mathematics, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK
Jonathan Thompson
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
Corresponding author
Correspondence to Saïd Salhi .
Editor information
Editors and affiliations.
Lancaster University, Lancaster, UK
John Boylan
Rights and permissions
Reprints and permissions
Copyright information
© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG
About this chapter
Salhi, S., Thompson, J. (2022). An Overview of Heuristics and Metaheuristics. In: Salhi, S., Boylan, J. (eds) The Palgrave Handbook of Operations Research . Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96935-6_11
Download citation
DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-96935-6_11
Published : 08 July 2022
Publisher Name : Palgrave Macmillan, Cham
Print ISBN : 978-3-030-96934-9
Online ISBN : 978-3-030-96935-6
eBook Packages : Business and Management Business and Management (R0)
Share this chapter
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
- Publish with us
Policies and ethics
- Find a journal
- Track your research
IMAGES
VIDEO
COMMENTS
Advantages and Disadvantages of Using Heuristics Advantages . ... a heuristic refers to a method of solving a problem that proves to be quicker or more efficient than traditional methods. This may ...
Heuristics aren't inherently good or bad, but there are pros and cons to using them to make decisions. While they can help us figure out a solution to a problem faster, they can also lead to inaccurate judgments about others or situations. Understanding these pros and cons may help you better use heuristics to make better decisions.
Advantages of Using Heuristic problem solving. Heuristic problem-solving strategies are the ones that use practical and intuitive methods to find solutions quickly, efficiently, and effectively. They can be applied to various problems and situations, from daily tasks to business or scientific problems. Here are some advantages of using ...
Heuristic evaluation tends to focus on fewer, more relevant areas so the problems it identifies tend to be important ones. Cons. The evaluation is only as good as the people you get to do it. This means you have to spend a lot of time analysing and reviewing experts to make sure they are relevant and experienced in the issues you are concerned ...
2. Next. A heuristic is a mental shortcut that allows an individual to make a decision, pass judgment, or solve a problem quickly and with minimal mental effort. While heuristics can reduce the ...
Heuristics are thinking or search strategies for problem solving that can help a problem solver in transforming the initial problematic situation progressively into a routine task for which he or she has the appropriate knowledge and skills to attain the intended goals, namely, the solution of the problem.
A heuristic is a word from the Greek meaning ‘to discover’. It is an approach to problem-solving that takes one’s personal experience into account. Heuristics provide strategies to scrutinize a limited number of signals and/or alternative choices in decision-making. Heuristics diminish the work of retrieving and storing information in ...
Heuristics and Problem Solving: Definitions, Benefits, and Limitations. The term heuristic, from the Greek, means, “serving to find out or discover”. (Todd and Gigerenzer, 2000, p. 738). In ...
Abstract. There are numerous optimisation problems for which heuristics are currently the only practical solution method. This chapter explains the need for heuristics before providing a brief overview of heuristic search techniques including metaheuristics. These include traditional local search methods including simulated annealing and tabu ...
A main advantage of heuristics is faster decision-making. The majority of people don't have time to undertake thorough research on every choice they are required to make. Heuristics help you to be more decisive and make choices based upon the knowledge you already have, which can be very useful when time is important.